BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

180 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 20045893)

  • 1. Modeling reaction time and accuracy of multiple-alternative decisions.
    Leite FP; Ratcliff R
    Atten Percept Psychophys; 2010 Jan; 72(1):246-73. PubMed ID: 20045893
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 2. Sequential sampling models without random between-trial variability: the racing diffusion model of speeded decision making.
    Tillman G; Van Zandt T; Logan GD
    Psychon Bull Rev; 2020 Oct; 27(5):911-936. PubMed ID: 32424622
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 3. Perceptual discrimination in static and dynamic noise: the temporal relation between perceptual encoding and decision making.
    Ratcliff R; Smith PL
    J Exp Psychol Gen; 2010 Feb; 139(1):70-94. PubMed ID: 20121313
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 4. A comparison of sequential sampling models for two-choice reaction time.
    Ratcliff R; Smith PL
    Psychol Rev; 2004 Apr; 111(2):333-67. PubMed ID: 15065913
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 5. Equal evidence perceptual tasks suggest a key role for interactive competition in decision-making.
    Kirkpatrick RP; Turner BM; Sederberg PB
    Psychol Rev; 2021 Nov; 128(6):1051-1087. PubMed ID: 34014711
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 6. Judging the difficulty of perceptual decisions.
    Löffler A; Zylberberg A; Shadlen MN; Wolpert DM
    Elife; 2023 Nov; 12():. PubMed ID: 37975792
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 7. The presence of irrelevant alternatives paradoxically increases confidence in perceptual decisions.
    Comay NA; Della Bella G; Lamberti P; Sigman M; Solovey G; Barttfeld P
    Cognition; 2023 May; 234():105377. PubMed ID: 36680974
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 8. A model of the go/no-go task.
    Gomez P; Ratcliff R; Perea M
    J Exp Psychol Gen; 2007 Aug; 136(3):389-413. PubMed ID: 17696690
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 9. Different effects of dopaminergic medication on perceptual decision-making in Parkinson's disease as a function of task difficulty and speed-accuracy instructions.
    Huang YT; Georgiev D; Foltynie T; Limousin P; Speekenbrink M; Jahanshahi M
    Neuropsychologia; 2015 Aug; 75():577-87. PubMed ID: 26184442
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 10. Modeling accuracy, response time, and bias in continuous orientation judgments.
    Kvam PD
    J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform; 2019 Mar; 45(3):301-318. PubMed ID: 30714760
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 11. What mechanisms mediate prior probability effects on rapid-choice decision-making?
    Puri R; Hinder MR; Heathcote A
    PLoS One; 2023; 18(7):e0288085. PubMed ID: 37418378
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 12. Perceptual and categorical decision making: goal-relevant representation of two domains at different levels of abstraction.
    Shankar S; Kayser AS
    J Neurophysiol; 2017 Jun; 117(6):2088-2103. PubMed ID: 28250149
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 13. Bayesian analysis of the piecewise diffusion decision model.
    Holmes WR; Trueblood JS
    Behav Res Methods; 2018 Apr; 50(2):730-743. PubMed ID: 28597236
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 14. A comparison of two diffusion process models in accounting for payoff and stimulus frequency manipulations.
    Leite FP
    Atten Percept Psychophys; 2012 Aug; 74(6):1366-82. PubMed ID: 22669791
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 15. Dissociating neural variability related to stimulus quality and response times in perceptual decision-making.
    Bode S; Bennett D; Sewell DK; Paton B; Egan GF; Smith PL; Murawski C
    Neuropsychologia; 2018 Mar; 111():190-200. PubMed ID: 29408524
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 16. A further test of sequential-sampling models that account for payoff effects on response bias in perceptual decision tasks.
    Diederich A
    Percept Psychophys; 2008 Feb; 70(2):229-56. PubMed ID: 18372746
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 17. Time-varying boundaries for diffusion models of decision making and response time.
    Zhang S; Lee MD; Vandekerckhove J; Maris G; Wagenmakers EJ
    Front Psychol; 2014; 5():1364. PubMed ID: 25538642
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 18. A Two-Stage Process Model of Sensory Discrimination: An Alternative to Drift-Diffusion.
    Sun P; Landy MS
    J Neurosci; 2016 Nov; 36(44):11259-11274. PubMed ID: 27807167
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 19. Cortical Components of Reaction-Time during Perceptual Decisions in Humans.
    Dmochowski JP; Norcia AM
    PLoS One; 2015; 10(11):e0143339. PubMed ID: 26599741
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 20. Sequential Sampling Models in Cognitive Neuroscience: Advantages, Applications, and Extensions.
    Forstmann BU; Ratcliff R; Wagenmakers EJ
    Annu Rev Psychol; 2016; 67():641-66. PubMed ID: 26393872
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Next]    [New Search]
    of 9.