BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

191 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 20116159)

  • 21. Using plausible group sizes to communicate information about medical risks.
    Garcia-Retamero R; Galesic M
    Patient Educ Couns; 2011 Aug; 84(2):245-50. PubMed ID: 20728298
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 22. Patient education and health literacy.
    Wittink H; Oosterhaven J
    Musculoskelet Sci Pract; 2018 Dec; 38():120-127. PubMed ID: 30017902
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 23. Preference for and understanding of graphs presenting health risk information. The role of age, health literacy, numeracy and graph literacy.
    van Weert JCM; Alblas MC; van Dijk L; Jansen J
    Patient Educ Couns; 2021 Jan; 104(1):109-117. PubMed ID: 32727670
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 24. Strengths and Gaps in Physicians' Risk Communication: A Scenario Study of the Influence of Numeracy on Cancer Screening Communication.
    Petrova D; Kostopoulou O; Delaney BC; Cokely ET; Garcia-Retamero R
    Med Decis Making; 2018 Apr; 38(3):355-365. PubMed ID: 28884617
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 25. Statistical numeracy for health: a cross-cultural comparison with probabilistic national samples.
    Galesic M; Garcia-Retamero R
    Arch Intern Med; 2010 Mar; 170(5):462-8. PubMed ID: 20212183
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 26. The effect of different styles of medical illustration on information comprehension, the perception of educational material and illness beliefs.
    Krasnoryadtseva A; Dalbeth N; Petrie KJ
    Patient Educ Couns; 2020 Mar; 103(3):556-562. PubMed ID: 31601448
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 27. Communicating Relative Risk Changes with Baseline Risk: Presentation Format and Numeracy Matter.
    Bodemer N; Meder B; Gigerenzer G
    Med Decis Making; 2014 Jul; 34(5):615-26. PubMed ID: 24803429
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 28. Comparing the impact of an icon array versus a bar graph on preference and understanding of risk information: Results from an online, randomized study.
    Scalia P; Schubbe DC; Lu ES; Durand MA; Frascara J; Noel G; O'Malley AJ; Elwyn G
    PLoS One; 2021; 16(7):e0253644. PubMed ID: 34297713
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 29. Relevance of graph literacy in the development of patient-centered communication tools.
    Nayak JG; Hartzler AL; Macleod LC; Izard JP; Dalkin BM; Gore JL
    Patient Educ Couns; 2016 Mar; 99(3):448-454. PubMed ID: 26481910
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 30. How Do People Process Different Representations of Statistical Information? Insights into Cognitive Effort, Representational Inconsistencies, and Individual Differences.
    Tiede KE; Gaissmaier W
    Med Decis Making; 2023; 43(7-8):803-820. PubMed ID: 37842816
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 31. Visual representation of statistical information improves diagnostic inferences in doctors and their patients.
    Garcia-Retamero R; Hoffrage U
    Soc Sci Med; 2013 Apr; 83():27-33. PubMed ID: 23465201
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 32. Measuring numeracy without a math test: development of the Subjective Numeracy Scale.
    Fagerlin A; Zikmund-Fisher BJ; Ubel PA; Jankovic A; Derry HA; Smith DM
    Med Decis Making; 2007; 27(5):672-80. PubMed ID: 17641137
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 33. Using animated computer-generated text and graphics to depict the risks and benefits of medical treatment.
    Tait AR; Voepel-Lewis T; Brennan-Martinez C; McGonegal M; Levine R
    Am J Med; 2012 Nov; 125(11):1103-10. PubMed ID: 22939094
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 34. Improving risk understanding across ability levels: Encouraging active processing with dynamic icon arrays.
    Okan Y; Garcia-Retamero R; Cokely ET; Maldonado A
    J Exp Psychol Appl; 2015 Jun; 21(2):178-94. PubMed ID: 25938975
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 35. Visual aids improve diagnostic inferences and metacognitive judgment calibration.
    Garcia-Retamero R; Cokely ET; Hoffrage U
    Front Psychol; 2015; 6():932. PubMed ID: 26236247
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 36. Communicating risk information: the influence of graphical display format on quantitative information perception-Accuracy, comprehension and preferences.
    Price M; Cameron R; Butow P
    Patient Educ Couns; 2007 Dec; 69(1-3):121-8. PubMed ID: 17905553
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 37. Numbers can be worth a thousand pictures: individual differences in understanding graphical and numerical representations of health-related information.
    Gaissmaier W; Wegwarth O; Skopec D; Müller AS; Broschinski S; Politi MC
    Health Psychol; 2012 May; 31(3):286-96. PubMed ID: 21842998
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 38. Does Guiding Toward Task-Relevant Information Help Improve Graph Processing and Graph Comprehension of Individuals with Low or High Numeracy? An Eye-Tracker Experiment.
    Keller C; Junghans A
    Med Decis Making; 2017 Nov; 37(8):942-954. PubMed ID: 28618918
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 39. Design, development, and evaluation of visual aids for communicating prescription drug instructions to nonliterate patients in rural Cameroon.
    Ngoh LN; Shepherd MD
    Patient Educ Couns; 1997 Jul; 31(3):245-61. PubMed ID: 9277247
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 40. The influence of graphic display format on the interpretations of quantitative risk information among adults with lower education and literacy: a randomized experimental study.
    McCaffery KJ; Dixon A; Hayen A; Jansen J; Smith S; Simpson JM
    Med Decis Making; 2012; 32(4):532-44. PubMed ID: 22074912
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Previous]   [Next]    [New Search]
    of 10.