These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

216 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 20472514)

  • 1. Developmental robotics modeling of the functions of language in action learning: reply to comment on "Grounding language in action and perception: from cognitive agents to humanoid robots".
    Cangelosi A
    Phys Life Rev; 2010 Jun; 7(2):154-5. PubMed ID: 20472514
    [No Abstract]   [Full Text] [Related]  

  • 2. From action to language and back Comment on 'Grounding language in action and perception: from cognitive agents to humanoid robots' by Cangelosi.
    Rohlfing KJ; Wrede B
    Phys Life Rev; 2010 Jun; 7(2):152-3. PubMed ID: 20444659
    [No Abstract]   [Full Text] [Related]  

  • 3. Grounding language in action and perception: from cognitive agents to humanoid robots.
    Cangelosi A
    Phys Life Rev; 2010 Jun; 7(2):139-51. PubMed ID: 20416855
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 4. The grounding of higher order concepts in action and language: a cognitive robotics model.
    Stramandinoli F; Marocco D; Cangelosi A
    Neural Netw; 2012 Aug; 32():165-73. PubMed ID: 22386502
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 5. The dynamic neural field approach to cognitive robotics.
    Erlhagen W; Bicho E
    J Neural Eng; 2006 Sep; 3(3):R36-54. PubMed ID: 16921201
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 6. Grounding words in perception and action: computational insights.
    Roy D
    Trends Cogn Sci; 2005 Aug; 9(8):389-96. PubMed ID: 16006171
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 7. Action and language integration: from humans to cognitive robots.
    Borghi AM; Cangelosi A
    Top Cogn Sci; 2014 Jul; 6(3):344-58. PubMed ID: 24943900
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 8. The cognitive nature of action - functional links between cognitive psychology, movement science, and robotics.
    Schack T; Ritter H
    Prog Brain Res; 2009; 174():231-50. PubMed ID: 19477343
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 9. Emergence and development of embodied cognition: a constructivist approach using robots.
    Kuniyoshi Y; Yorozu Y; Suzuki S; Sangawa S; Ohmura Y; Terada K; Nagakubo A
    Prog Brain Res; 2007; 164():425-45. PubMed ID: 17920445
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 10. From embodied mind to embodied robotics: humanities and system theoretical aspects.
    Mainzer K
    J Physiol Paris; 2009; 103(3-5):296-304. PubMed ID: 19665560
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 11. The ITALK project: a developmental robotics approach to the study of individual, social, and linguistic learning.
    Broz F; Nehaniv CL; Belpaeme T; Bisio A; Dautenhahn K; Fadiga L; Ferrauto T; Fischer K; Förster F; Gigliotta O; Griffiths S; Lehmann H; Lohan KS; Lyon C; Marocco D; Massera G; Metta G; Mohan V; Morse A; Nolfi S; Nori F; Peniak M; Pitsch K; Rohlfing KJ; Sagerer G; Sato Y; Saunders J; Schillingmann L; Sciutti A; Tikhanoff V; Wrede B; Zeschel A; Cangelosi A
    Top Cogn Sci; 2014 Jul; 6(3):534-44. PubMed ID: 24934294
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 12. Modeling developmental cognitive neuroscience.
    Westermann G; Sirois S; Shultz TR; Mareschal D
    Trends Cogn Sci; 2006 May; 10(5):227-32. PubMed ID: 16603407
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 13. Can we talk to robots? Ten-month-old infants expected interactive humanoid robots to be talked to by persons.
    Arita A; Hiraki K; Kanda T; Ishiguro H
    Cognition; 2005 Apr; 95(3):B49-57. PubMed ID: 15788157
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 14. Computational motor control in humans and robots.
    Schaal S; Schweighofer N
    Curr Opin Neurobiol; 2005 Dec; 15(6):675-82. PubMed ID: 16271466
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 15. Activation without selection: parallel right hemisphere roles in language and intentional movement?
    Richards L; Chiarello C
    Brain Lang; 1997 Mar; 57(1):151-78. PubMed ID: 9126411
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 16. An embodied model for sensorimotor grounding and grounding transfer: experiments with epigenetic robots.
    Cangelosi A; Riga T
    Cogn Sci; 2006 Jul; 30(4):673-89. PubMed ID: 21702830
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 17. A computational model of perception and action for cognitive robotics.
    Haazebroek P; van Dantzig S; Hommel B
    Cogn Process; 2011 Nov; 12(4):355-65. PubMed ID: 21597926
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 18. Motor cognition: a new paradigm to study self-other interactions.
    Jackson PL; Decety J
    Curr Opin Neurobiol; 2004 Apr; 14(2):259-63. PubMed ID: 15082334
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 19. Social cognitive neuroscience and humanoid robotics.
    Chaminade T; Cheng G
    J Physiol Paris; 2009; 103(3-5):286-95. PubMed ID: 19665550
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 20. Biases and optimality of sensory-motor and cognitive decisions.
    Trommershäuser J
    Prog Brain Res; 2009; 174():267-78. PubMed ID: 19477345
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Next]    [New Search]
    of 11.