These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

143 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 20565206)

  • 1. Dissociations between expectancy and performance in simple and two-choice reaction-time tasks: A test of associative and nonassociative explanations.
    Barrett LC; Livesey EJ
    J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn; 2010 Jul; 36(4):864-77. PubMed ID: 20565206
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 2. Do reaction times in the Perruchet effect reflect variations in the strength of an associative link?
    Mitchell CJ; Wardle SG; Lovibond PF; Weidemann G; Chang BP
    J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn; 2010 Mar; 36(2):567-72. PubMed ID: 20192552
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 3. The influence of temporal factors on automatic priming and conscious expectancy in a simple reaction time task.
    Destrebecqz A; Perruchet P; Cleeremans A; Laureys S; Maquet P; Peigneux P
    Q J Exp Psychol (Hove); 2010 Feb; 63(2):291-309. PubMed ID: 19526437
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 4. Dissociating the effects of automatic activation and explicit expectancy on reaction times in a simple associative learning task.
    Perruchet P; Cleeremans A; Destrebecqz A
    J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn; 2006 Sep; 32(5):955-65. PubMed ID: 16938039
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 5. Testing the limits of the Perruchet effect in choice response time tasks.
    Lee Cheong Lem VA; Harris JA; Livesey EJ
    J Exp Psychol Anim Learn Cogn; 2015 Oct; 41(4):385-94. PubMed ID: 26301613
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 6. Human sequence learning under incidental and intentional conditions.
    Jones FW; McLaren IP
    J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process; 2009 Oct; 35(4):538-53. PubMed ID: 19839706
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 7. Automaticity and conscious control in single and choice response time versions of the Perruchet effect.
    Livesey EJ; Costa DS
    Q J Exp Psychol (Hove); 2014; 67(4):646-64. PubMed ID: 23972053
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 8. Equivalency of reaction times for simple and primed tasks.
    Hart MA; Reeve TG
    Acta Psychol (Amst); 2007 Jul; 125(3):291-300. PubMed ID: 17055991
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 9. Reaction time as a measure of human associative learning.
    Craddock P; Molet M; Miller RR
    Behav Processes; 2012 Jun; 90(2):189-97. PubMed ID: 22301095
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 10. Is Perruchet's dissociation between eyeblink conditioned responding and outcome expectancy evidence for two learning systems?
    Weidemann G; Tangen JM; Lovibond PF; Mitchell CJ
    J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process; 2009 Apr; 35(2):169-76. PubMed ID: 19364226
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 11. Associations or repetitions? Testing the basis of the Perruchet effect in voluntary response speed.
    Lee Cheong Lem VA; Moul C; Harris JA; Livesey EJ
    J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn; 2018 Dec; 44(12):1971-1985. PubMed ID: 29683708
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 12. Trace eyeblink conditioning in abstinent alcoholic individuals: effects of complex task demands and prior conditioning.
    McGlinchey RE; Fortier CB; Capozzi SM; Disterhoft JF
    Neuropsychology; 2005 Mar; 19(2):159-70. PubMed ID: 15769200
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 13. Dissociating expectancy of shock and changes in skin conductance: an investigation of the Perruchet effect using an electrodermal paradigm.
    McAndrew A; Jones FW; McLaren RP; McLaren IP
    J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process; 2012 Apr; 38(2):203-8. PubMed ID: 22250788
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 14. Saving the Perruchet effect: A role for the strength of the association in associative learning.
    Destrebecqz A; Vande Velde M; San Anton E; Cleeremans A; Bertels J
    Q J Exp Psychol (Hove); 2019 Jun; 72(6):1379-1386. PubMed ID: 29991319
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 15. Contextual cues aid recovery from interruption: the role of associative activation.
    Hodgetts HM; Jones DM
    J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn; 2006 Sep; 32(5):1120-32. PubMed ID: 16938050
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 16. Reorienting when cues conflict: A role for information content in spatial learning?
    Sturz BR; Diemer SM
    Behav Processes; 2010 Jan; 83(1):90-8. PubMed ID: 19903512
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 17. Interference between outcomes, spontaneous recovery, and context effects as measured by a cued response reaction time task: evidence for associative retrieval models.
    González-Martín E; Cobos PL; Morís J; López FJ
    J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process; 2012 Oct; 38(4):419-32. PubMed ID: 22905829
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 18. Heightened conflict in cue-target translation increases backward inhibition in set switching.
    Grange JA; Houghton G
    J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn; 2010 Jul; 36(4):1003-9. PubMed ID: 20565215
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 19. Recovery from blocking between outcomes.
    Wheeler DS; Miller RR
    J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process; 2005 Oct; 31(4):467-76. PubMed ID: 16248732
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 20. Learning about cues in their absence: evidence from flavour preferences and aversions.
    Dwyer DM
    Q J Exp Psychol B; 2003 Feb; 56(1):56-67. PubMed ID: 12623537
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Next]    [New Search]
    of 8.