BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

98 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 20584399)

  • 1. Not different kinds, just special cases.
    Danks D
    Behav Brain Sci; 2010 Jun; 33(2-3):208-9. PubMed ID: 20584399
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 2. Defending the concept of "concepts".
    Hayes BK; Kearney L
    Behav Brain Sci; 2010 Jun; 33(2-3):214. PubMed ID: 20584405
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 3. Default knowledge, time pressure, and the theory-theory of concepts.
    Blanchard T
    Behav Brain Sci; 2010 Jun; 33(2-3):206-7. PubMed ID: 20584397
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 4. Are prototypes and exemplars used in distinct cognitive processes?
    Virtel J; Piccinini G
    Behav Brain Sci; 2010 Jun; 33(2-3):226-7. PubMed ID: 20584418
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 5. Concepts are a functional kind.
    Lalumera E
    Behav Brain Sci; 2010 Jun; 33(2-3):217-8. PubMed ID: 20584409
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 6. Parsimony and the triple-system model of concepts.
    Zaki S; Cruz J
    Behav Brain Sci; 2010 Jun; 33(2-3):230-1. PubMed ID: 20584422
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 7. Conceptual atomism rethought.
    Schneider S
    Behav Brain Sci; 2010 Jun; 33(2-3):224-5. PubMed ID: 20584416
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 8. The function and representation of concepts.
    Khemlani SS; Goodwin G
    Behav Brain Sci; 2010 Jun; 33(2-3):216-7. PubMed ID: 20584408
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 9. From conceptual representations to explanatory relations.
    Lombrozo T
    Behav Brain Sci; 2010 Jun; 33(2-3):218-9. PubMed ID: 20584410
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 10. Unity amidst heterogeneity in theories of concepts.
    Edwards K
    Behav Brain Sci; 2010 Jun; 33(2-3):210-1. PubMed ID: 20584401
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 11. An additional heterogeneity hypothesis.
    Dove G
    Behav Brain Sci; 2010 Jun; 33(2-3):209-10. PubMed ID: 20584400
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 12. The faux, fake, forged, false, fabricated, and phony: problems for the independence of similarity-based theories of concepts.
    Jacobson AJ
    Behav Brain Sci; 2010 Jun; 33(2-3):215. PubMed ID: 20584407
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 13. Two uneliminated uses for "concepts": hybrids and guides for inquiry.
    Gonnerman C; Weinberg JM
    Behav Brain Sci; 2010 Jun; 33(2-3):211-2. PubMed ID: 20584402
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 14. Structured statistical models of inductive reasoning.
    Kemp C; Tenenbaum JB
    Psychol Rev; 2009 Jan; 116(1):20-58. PubMed ID: 19159147
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 15. PrĂ©cis of doing without concepts.
    Machery E
    Behav Brain Sci; 2010 Jun; 33(2-3):195-206; discussion 206-44. PubMed ID: 20584396
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 16. The acquisition of Boolean concepts.
    Goodwin GP; Johnson-Laird PN
    Trends Cogn Sci; 2013 Mar; 17(3):128-33. PubMed ID: 23428936
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 17. The theoretical indispensability of concepts.
    Weiskopf DA
    Behav Brain Sci; 2010 Jun; 33(2-3):228-9. PubMed ID: 20584420
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 18. Where are nature's joints? Finding the mechanisms underlying categorization.
    Markman AB
    Behav Brain Sci; 2010 Jun; 33(2-3):220-1. PubMed ID: 20584412
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 19. Rethinking concept analysis.
    Risjord M
    J Adv Nurs; 2009 Mar; 65(3):684-91. PubMed ID: 19222666
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 20. Universal moral grammar: theory, evidence and the future.
    Mikhail J
    Trends Cogn Sci; 2007 Apr; 11(4):143-52. PubMed ID: 17329147
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Next]    [New Search]
    of 5.