These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

177 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 20708664)

  • 1. Cognitive representation in transitive inference: a comparison of four corvid species.
    Bond AB; Wei CA; Kamil AC
    Behav Processes; 2010 Oct; 85(3):283-92. PubMed ID: 20708664
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 2. Abstract-concept learning in two species of new world corvids, pinyon jays (Gymnorhinus Cyanocephalus) and California scrub jays (Aphelocoma Californica).
    Vernouillet A; Leonard K; Katz JS; Magnotti JF; Wright A; Kelly DM
    J Exp Psychol Anim Learn Cogn; 2021 Jul; 47(3):384-392. PubMed ID: 34081496
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 3. Serial reversal learning and the evolution of behavioral flexibility in three species of North American corvids (Gymnorhinus cyanocephalus, Nucifraga columbiana, Aphelocoma californica).
    Bond AB; Kamil AC; Balda RP
    J Comp Psychol; 2007 Nov; 121(4):372-9. PubMed ID: 18085920
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 4. Performance of four seed-caching corvid species in the radial-arm maze analog.
    Kamil AC; Balda RP; Olson DJ
    J Comp Psychol; 1994 Dec; 108(4):385-93. PubMed ID: 7813195
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 5. Individual exploratory responses are not repeatable across time or context for four species of food-storing corvid.
    Vernouillet A; Kelly DM
    Sci Rep; 2020 Jan; 10(1):394. PubMed ID: 31941921
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 6. A comparison of four corvid species in a working and reference memory task using a radial maze.
    Gould-Beierle K
    J Comp Psychol; 2000 Dec; 114(4):347-56. PubMed ID: 11149538
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 7. Sociality and social learning in two species of corvids: the pinyon jay (Gymnorhinus cyanocephalus) and the Clark's nutcracker (Nucifraga columbiana).
    Templeton JJ; Kamil AC; Balda RP
    J Comp Psychol; 1999 Dec; 113(4):450-5. PubMed ID: 10608569
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 8. Direct and relational representation during transitive list linking in pinyon jays (Gymnorhinus cyanocephalus).
    Wei CA; Kamil AC; Bond AB
    J Comp Psychol; 2014 Feb; 128(1):1-10. PubMed ID: 24188622
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 9. Differences in hippocampal volume among food storing corvids.
    Basil JA; Kamil AC; Balda RP; Fite KV
    Brain Behav Evol; 1996; 47(3):156-64. PubMed ID: 8680849
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 10. Clark's nutcrackers (Nucifraga columbiana) are sensitive to distance, but not lighting when caching in the presence of a conspecific.
    Tornick JK; Rushia SN; Gibson BM
    Behav Processes; 2016 Feb; 123():125-33. PubMed ID: 26581321
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 11. Differences in relative hippocampus volume and number of hippocampus neurons among five corvid species.
    Gould KL; Gilbertson KE; Hrvol AJ; Nelson JC; Seyfer AL; Brantner RM; Kamil AC
    Brain Behav Evol; 2013; 81(1):56-70. PubMed ID: 23364270
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 12. Clark's nutcrackers (Nucifraga columbiana) use a visual barrier for cache protection.
    Tornick J; Gibson B
    J Comp Psychol; 2021 May; 135(2):170-175. PubMed ID: 33444031
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 13. The control of food-caching behavior by Western scrub-jays (Aphelocoma californica).
    de Kort SR; Correia SP; Alexis DM; Dickinson A; Clayton NS
    J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process; 2007 Oct; 33(4):361-70. PubMed ID: 17924785
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 14. Cache protection strategies of a non-social food-caching corvid, Clark's nutcracker (Nucifraga columbiana).
    Clary D; Kelly DM
    Anim Cogn; 2011 Sep; 14(5):735-44. PubMed ID: 21538135
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 15. Clark's nutcracker spatial memory: the importance of large, structural cues.
    Bednekoff PA; Balda RP
    Behav Processes; 2014 Feb; 102():12-7. PubMed ID: 24333225
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 16. Examination of long-term visual memorization capacity in the Clark's nutcracker (Nucifraga columbiana).
    Qadri MAJ; Leonard K; Cook RG; Kelly DM
    Psychon Bull Rev; 2018 Dec; 25(6):2274-2280. PubMed ID: 29450792
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 17. Is the western scrub-jay (Aphelocoma californica) really an underdog among food-caching corvids when it comes to hippocampal volume and food caching propensity?
    Pravosudov VV; de Kort SR
    Brain Behav Evol; 2006; 67(1):1-9. PubMed ID: 16219995
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 18. Clark's Nutcrackers (
    Clary D; Kelly DM
    Front Psychol; 2016; 7():1643. PubMed ID: 27826273
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 19. Tests of inferential reasoning by exclusion in Clark's nutcrackers (Nucifraga columbiana).
    Tornick JK; Gibson BM
    Anim Cogn; 2013 Jul; 16(4):583-97. PubMed ID: 23338970
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 20. Superior abstract-concept learning by Clark's nutcrackers (Nucifraga columbiana).
    Magnotti JF; Katz JS; Wright AA; Kelly DM
    Biol Lett; 2015 May; 11(5):20150148. PubMed ID: 25972399
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Next]    [New Search]
    of 9.