These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

209 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 20853967)

  • 1. Age and individual differences in prospective memory during a "Virtual Week": the roles of working memory, vigilance, task regularity, and cue focality.
    Rose NS; Rendell PG; McDaniel MA; Aberle I; Kliegel M
    Psychol Aging; 2010 Sep; 25(3):595-605. PubMed ID: 20853967
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 2. Prospective memory and intraindividual variability in ongoing task response times in an adult lifespan sample: the role of cue focality.
    Ihle A; Ghisletta P; Kliegel M
    Memory; 2017 Mar; 25(3):370-376. PubMed ID: 27088610
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 3. Prospective memory in Parkinson disease during a virtual week: effects of both prospective and retrospective demands.
    Foster ER; Rose NS; McDaniel MA; Rendell PG
    Neuropsychology; 2013 Mar; 27(2):170-81. PubMed ID: 23527645
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 4. Adult age differences, response management, and cue focality in event-based prospective memory: a meta-analysis on the role of task order specificity.
    Ihle A; Hering A; Mahy CE; Bisiacchi PS; Kliegel M
    Psychol Aging; 2013 Sep; 28(3):714-20. PubMed ID: 24041004
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 5. Auditory temporal preparation induced by rhythmic cues during concurrent auditory working memory tasks.
    Cutanda D; Correa Á; Sanabria D
    J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform; 2015 Jun; 41(3):790-7. PubMed ID: 25893682
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 6. The interplay of intention maintenance and cue monitoring in younger and older adults' prospective memory.
    Ballhausen N; Schnitzspahn KM; Horn SS; Kliegel M
    Mem Cognit; 2017 Oct; 45(7):1113-1125. PubMed ID: 28600628
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 7. The age prospective memory paradox: young adults may not give their best outside of the lab.
    Aberle I; Rendell PG; Rose NS; McDaniel MA; Kliegel M
    Dev Psychol; 2010 Nov; 46(6):1444-53. PubMed ID: 21058832
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 8. Response dynamics in prospective memory.
    Abney DH; McBride DM; Conte AM; Vinson DW
    Psychon Bull Rev; 2015 Aug; 22(4):1020-8. PubMed ID: 25407659
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 9. No age deficits in the ability to use attention to improve visual working memory.
    Souza AS
    Psychol Aging; 2016 Aug; 31(5):456-70. PubMed ID: 27253868
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 10. Investigating prospective memory via eye tracking: No evidence for a monitoring deficit in older adults.
    Ballhausen N; Lauffs MM; Herzog MH; Kliegel M
    Int J Psychophysiol; 2019 Dec; 146():107-116. PubMed ID: 31655183
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 11. Aging and prospective memory: the role of working memory and monitoring processes.
    Bisiacchi PS; Tarantino V; Ciccola A
    Aging Clin Exp Res; 2008 Dec; 20(6):569-77. PubMed ID: 19179842
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 12. The development of prospective memory in preschool children using naturalistic tasks.
    Walsh SJ; Martin GM; Courage ML
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2014 Nov; 127():8-23. PubMed ID: 24290293
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 13. The flexible engagement of monitoring processes in non-focal and focal prospective memory tasks with salient cues.
    Hefer C; Cohen AL; Jaudas A; Dreisbach G
    Acta Psychol (Amst); 2017 Sep; 179():42-53. PubMed ID: 28711672
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 14. Prospective memory and aging: evidence for preserved spontaneous retrieval with exact but not related cues.
    Mullet HG; Scullin MK; Hess TJ; Scullin RB; Arnold KM; Einstein GO
    Psychol Aging; 2013 Dec; 28(4):910-22. PubMed ID: 24364398
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 15. Transparent meta-analysis: does aging spare prospective memory with focal vs. non-focal cues?
    Uttl B
    PLoS One; 2011 Feb; 6(2):e16618. PubMed ID: 21304905
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 16. An individual difference perspective on focal versus nonfocal prospective memory.
    Zuber S; Kliegel M; Ihle A
    Mem Cognit; 2016 Nov; 44(8):1192-1203. PubMed ID: 27351880
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 17. The effects of age, enactment, and cue-action relatedness on memory for intentions in the Virtual Week task.
    Pereira A; Ellis JA; Freeman JE
    Neuropsychol Dev Cogn B Aging Neuropsychol Cogn; 2012 Sep; 19(5):549-65. PubMed ID: 22221208
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 18. A thorough examination of cue specificity and task-appropriateness in defining focal and nonfocal prospective memory tasks.
    Lyon BA; Hicks JL
    Memory; 2023 May; 31(5):665-677. PubMed ID: 36945870
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 19. Behavioral and Neural Markers of Flexible Attention over Working Memory in Aging.
    Mok RM; Myers NE; Wallis G; Nobre AC
    Cereb Cortex; 2016 Apr; 26(4):1831-42. PubMed ID: 26865653
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 20. Using Virtual Week to assess prospective memory in younger and older adults.
    Mioni G; Stablum F; Rendell PG
    Stud Health Technol Inform; 2012; 181():118-22. PubMed ID: 22954840
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Next]    [New Search]
    of 11.