These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

91 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 21044665)

  • 1. Testing brown lemurs (Eulemur fulvus) on the reverse-reward contingency task without a modified procedure.
    Genty E; Chung PC; Roeder JJ
    Behav Processes; 2011 Jan; 86(1):133-7. PubMed ID: 21044665
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 2. Transfer of self-control in black (Eulemur macaco) and brown (Eulemur fulvus) lemurs: choice of a less preferred food item under a reverse-reward contingency.
    Genty E; Roeder JJ
    J Comp Psychol; 2007 Nov; 121(4):354-62. PubMed ID: 18085918
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 3. Can lemurs (Eulemur fulvus and E. macaco) use abstract representations of quantities to master the reverse-reward contingency task?
    Genty E; Roeder JJ
    Primates; 2011 Jul; 52(3):253-60. PubMed ID: 21394424
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 4. Mangabeys (Cercocebus torquatus lunulatus) solve the reverse contingency task without a modified procedure.
    Albiach-Serrano A; Guillén-Salazar F; Call J
    Anim Cogn; 2007 Oct; 10(4):387-96. PubMed ID: 17318622
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 5. Brown lemurs (Eulemur fulvus) can master the qualitative version of the reverse-reward contingency.
    Glady Y; Genty É; Roeder JJ
    PLoS One; 2012; 7(10):e48378. PubMed ID: 23118998
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 6. Rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta) spontaneously generalize to novel quantities in a reverse-reward contingency task.
    Kralik JD
    J Comp Psychol; 2012 Aug; 126(3):255-62. PubMed ID: 22122399
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 7. Do cleaner fish learn to feed against their preference in a reverse reward contingency task?
    Danisman E; Bshary R; Bergmüller R
    Anim Cogn; 2010 Jan; 13(1):41-9. PubMed ID: 19533186
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 8. How prosimian primates represent tools: experiments with two lemur species (Eulemur fulvus and Lemur catta).
    Santos LR; Mahajan N; Barnes JL
    J Comp Psychol; 2005 Nov; 119(4):394-403. PubMed ID: 16366773
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 9. How the great apes (Pan troglodytes, Pongo pygmaeus, Pan paniscus, Gorilla gorilla) perform on the reversed reward contingency task II: transfer to new quantities, long-term retention, and the impact of quantity ratios.
    Uher J; Call J
    J Comp Psychol; 2008 May; 122(2):204-12. PubMed ID: 18489236
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 10. Quality before quantity: rapid learning of reverse-reward contingency by capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella).
    Anderson JR; Hattori Y; Fujita K
    J Comp Psychol; 2008 Nov; 122(4):445-8. PubMed ID: 19014269
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 11. Can brown lemurs (Eulemur fulvus) learn to deceive a human competitor?
    Genty E; Foltz J; Roeder JJ
    Anim Cogn; 2008 Apr; 11(2):255-66. PubMed ID: 17876614
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 12. Transitive inference in two lemur species (Eulemur macaco and Eulemur fulvus).
    Tromp D; Meunier H; Roeder JJ
    Am J Primatol; 2015 Mar; 77(3):338-45. PubMed ID: 25328141
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 13. Any reward will do: Effects of a reverse-reward contingency on size preference with pet dogs (Canis lupus familiaris).
    Fernand JK; Amanieh H; Cox DJ; Dorey NR
    Learn Behav; 2018 Dec; 46(4):472-478. PubMed ID: 30120727
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 14. Brown (Eulemur fulvus) and ring-tailed lemurs (Lemur catta) use human head orientation as a cue to gaze direction in a food choice task.
    Botting JL; Wiper ML; Anderson JR
    Folia Primatol (Basel); 2011; 82(3):165-76. PubMed ID: 22123170
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 15. Can lemurs learn to deceive? A study in the black lemur (Eulemur macaco).
    Genty E; Roeder JJ
    J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process; 2006 Apr; 32(2):196-200. PubMed ID: 16634664
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 16. Inferences about the location of food in lemurs (Eulemur macaco and Eulemur fulvus): a comparison with apes and monkeys.
    Maille A; Roeder JJ
    Anim Cogn; 2012 Nov; 15(6):1075-83. PubMed ID: 22806271
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 17. Intertemporal choice in lemurs.
    Stevens JR; Mühlhoff N
    Behav Processes; 2012 Feb; 89(2):121-7. PubMed ID: 22024661
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 18. Squirrel monkeys (Saimiri sciureus) choose smaller food arrays: long-term retention, choice with nonpreferred food, and transposition.
    Anderson JR; Awazu S; Fujita K
    J Comp Psychol; 2004 Mar; 118(1):58-64. PubMed ID: 15008673
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 19. Rhesus monkeys with orbital prefrontal cortex lesions can learn to inhibit prepotent responses in the reversed reward contingency task.
    Chudasama Y; Kralik JD; Murray EA
    Cereb Cortex; 2007 May; 17(5):1154-9. PubMed ID: 16774961
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 20. Enumeration of objects and substances in non-human primates: experiments with brown lemurs (Eulemur fulvus).
    Mahajan N; Barnes JL; Blanco M; Santos LR
    Dev Sci; 2009 Nov; 12(6):920-8. PubMed ID: 19840047
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Next]    [New Search]
    of 5.