These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

129 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 21121470)

  • 61. That's my teacher! Children's ability to recognize personally familiar and unfamiliar faces improves with age.
    Laurence S; Mondloch CJ
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2016 Mar; 143():123-38. PubMed ID: 26530253
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 62. Young children's performance on a task sensitive to the memory functions of the medial temporal lobe in adults--the delayed nonmatching-to-sample task--reveals problems that are due to non-memory-related task demands.
    Diamond A; Towle C; Boyer K
    Behav Neurosci; 1994 Aug; 108(4):659-80. PubMed ID: 7986361
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 63. Conceptually coherent categories support label-based inductive generalization in preschoolers.
    Booth AE
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2014 Jul; 123():1-14. PubMed ID: 24632505
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 64. Young children's understanding of the context-relativity of normative rules in conventional games.
    Rakoczy H; Brosche N; Warneken F; Tomasello M
    Br J Dev Psychol; 2009 Jun; 27(Pt 2):445-56. PubMed ID: 19998540
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 65. Perceptual development and category generalization.
    Smith LB
    Child Dev; 1979 Sep; 50(3):705-15. PubMed ID: 498847
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 66. Two-year-olds' sensitivity to subphonemic mismatch during online spoken word recognition.
    Paquette-Smith M; Fecher N; Johnson EK
    Atten Percept Psychophys; 2016 Nov; 78(8):2329-2340. PubMed ID: 27582281
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 67. Enabling conditions and children's understanding of pretense.
    Sobel DM
    Cognition; 2009 Nov; 113(2):177-88. PubMed ID: 19733841
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 68. Development of category formation for faces differing by age in 9- to 12-month-olds: An effect of experience with infant faces.
    Damon F; Quinn PC; Heron-Delaney M; Lee K; Pascalis O
    Br J Dev Psychol; 2016 Nov; 34(4):582-597. PubMed ID: 27393740
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 69. Developmental differences in part/whole identification.
    Prather PA; Bacon J
    Child Dev; 1986 Jun; 57(3):549-58. PubMed ID: 3720392
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 70. Making a silk purse out of two sow's ears: young children's use of comparison in category learning.
    Namy LL; Gentner D
    J Exp Psychol Gen; 2002 Mar; 131(1):5-15. PubMed ID: 11900103
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 71. Preschoolers' Thoughts on the Concept of Time.
    Güneş G; Şahin V
    J Genet Psychol; 2020; 181(4):293-317. PubMed ID: 32308148
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 72. How fantasy benefits young children's understanding of pretense.
    Sobel DM
    Dev Sci; 2006 Jan; 9(1):63-75. PubMed ID: 16445397
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 73. Developmental differences in the relative weighing of informants' social attributes.
    Reyes-Jaquez B; Echols CH
    Dev Psychol; 2013 Mar; 49(3):602-13. PubMed ID: 23356527
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 74. The Development of Selective Copying: Children's Learning From an Expert Versus Their Mother.
    Lucas AJ; Burdett ERR; Burgess V; Wood LA; McGuigan N; Harris PL; Whiten A
    Child Dev; 2017 Nov; 88(6):2026-2042. PubMed ID: 28032639
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 75. Reading to learn: prereaders' and early readers' trust in text as a source of knowledge.
    Robinson EJ; Einav S; Fox A
    Dev Psychol; 2013 Mar; 49(3):505-13. PubMed ID: 22822937
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 76. Distributing learning over time: the spacing effect in children's acquisition and generalization of science concepts.
    Vlach HA; Sandhofer CM
    Child Dev; 2012; 83(4):1137-44. PubMed ID: 22616822
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 77. The role of local and global processing in the recognition of living and nonliving things.
    Thomas R; Forde E
    Neuropsychologia; 2006; 44(6):982-6. PubMed ID: 16253294
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 78. Processing Demands Impact 3-Year-Olds' Performance in a Spontaneous-Response Task: New Evidence for the Processing-Load Account of Early False-Belief Understanding.
    Scott RM; Roby E
    PLoS One; 2015; 10(11):e0142405. PubMed ID: 26562840
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 79. What makes a vibble a vibble? A developmental study of category generalization.
    Ward TB; Vela E; Peery ML; Lewis SN; Bauer NK; Klint KA
    Child Dev; 1989 Feb; 60(1):214-24. PubMed ID: 2702869
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 80. Children's understanding of knowledge acquisition: the tendency for children to report that they have always known what they have just learned.
    Taylor M; Esbensen BM; Bennett RT
    Child Dev; 1994 Dec; 65(6):1581-604. PubMed ID: 7859544
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Previous]   [Next]    [New Search]
    of 7.