These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

384 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 21205671)

  • 41. Lifespan aging and belief reasoning: Influences of executive function and social cue decoding.
    Phillips LH; Bull R; Allen R; Insch P; Burr K; Ogg W
    Cognition; 2011 Aug; 120(2):236-47. PubMed ID: 21624567
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 42. Eighteen-month-olds understand false beliefs in an unexpected-contents task.
    Buttelmann D; Over H; Carpenter M; Tomasello M
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2014 Mar; 119():120-6. PubMed ID: 24267393
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 43. Does altercentric interference rely on mentalizing?: Results from two level-1 perspective-taking tasks.
    Marshall J; Gollwitzer A; Santos LR
    PLoS One; 2018; 13(3):e0194101. PubMed ID: 29566019
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 44. The cost of thinking about false beliefs: evidence from adults' performance on a non-inferential theory of mind task.
    Apperly IA; Back E; Samson D; France L
    Cognition; 2008 Mar; 106(3):1093-108. PubMed ID: 17662706
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 45. Eye movements reveal sustained implicit processing of others' mental states.
    Schneider D; Bayliss AP; Becker SI; Dux PE
    J Exp Psychol Gen; 2012 Aug; 141(3):433-8. PubMed ID: 21910557
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 46. Do you see what I see? Infants' reasoning about others' incomplete perceptions.
    Luo Y; Beck W
    Dev Sci; 2010 Jan; 13(1):134-42. PubMed ID: 20121869
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 47. Tracking speakers' false beliefs: is theory of mind available earlier for word learning?
    Houston-Price C; Goddard K; Séclier C; Grant SC; Reid CJ; Boyden LE; Williams R
    Dev Sci; 2011 Jul; 14(4):623-34. PubMed ID: 21676084
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 48. Representing how rabbits quack and competitors act: limits on preschoolers' efficient ability to track perspective.
    Low J; Drummond W; Walmsley A; Wang B
    Child Dev; 2014; 85(4):1519-34. PubMed ID: 24476382
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 49. Infants attribute false beliefs to a toy crane.
    Burnside K; Severdija V; Poulin-Dubois D
    Dev Sci; 2020 Mar; 23(2):e12887. PubMed ID: 31309631
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 50. What Could Go Wrong: Adults and Children Calibrate Predictions and Explanations of Others' Actions Based on Relative Reward and Danger.
    Gjata NN; Ullman TD; Spelke ES; Liu S
    Cogn Sci; 2022 Jul; 46(7):e13163. PubMed ID: 35738555
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 51. Is Language Required to Represent Others' Mental States? Evidence From Beliefs and Other Representations.
    Samuel S; Durdevic K; Legg EW; Lurz R; Clayton NS
    Cogn Sci; 2019 Jan; 43(1):. PubMed ID: 30648802
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 52. Seeing the World From Others' Perspective: 14-Month-Olds Show Altercentric Modulation Effects by Others' Beliefs.
    Kampis D; Kovács ÁM
    Open Mind (Camb); 2021; 5():189-207. PubMed ID: 36438424
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 53. Principles of belief acquisition. How we read other minds.
    Pascarelli MT; Quarona D; Barchiesi G; Riva G; Butterfill SA; Sinigaglia C
    Conscious Cogn; 2024 Jan; 117():103625. PubMed ID: 38159535
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 54. Nine-months-old infants do not need to know what the agent prefers in order to reason about its goals: on the role of preference and persistence in infants' goal-attribution.
    Hernik M; Southgate V
    Dev Sci; 2012 Sep; 15(5):714-22. PubMed ID: 22925518
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 55. Do 8-Month-Old Infants Consider Situational Constraints When Interpreting Others' Gaze as Goal-Directed Action?
    Luo Y
    Infancy; 2010 Jul; 15(4):392-419. PubMed ID: 32693520
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 56. Level 2 perspective-taking distinguishes automatic and non-automatic belief-tracking.
    Edwards K; Low J
    Cognition; 2019 Dec; 193():104017. PubMed ID: 31271926
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 57. Children with autism can track others' beliefs in a competitive game.
    Peterson CC; Slaughter V; Peterson J; Premack D
    Dev Sci; 2013 May; 16(3):443-50. PubMed ID: 23587041
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 58. Belief attribution in deaf and hearing infants.
    Meristo M; Morgan G; Geraci A; Iozzi L; Hjelmquist E; Surian L; Siegal M
    Dev Sci; 2012 Sep; 15(5):633-40. PubMed ID: 22925511
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 59. The effects of adults' affective expression and direction of visual gaze on 12-month-olds' visual preferences for an object following a 5-minute, 1-day, or 1-month delay.
    Flom R; Johnson S
    Br J Dev Psychol; 2011 Mar; 29(Pt 1):64-85. PubMed ID: 21288254
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 60. Does social distance modulate adults' egocentric biases when reasoning about false beliefs?
    Farrar BG; Ostojić L
    PLoS One; 2018; 13(6):e0198616. PubMed ID: 29883498
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Previous]   [Next]    [New Search]
    of 20.