These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

103 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 21310170)

  • 1. Vision-for-perception and vision-for-action: which model is compatible with the available psychophysical and neuropsychological data?
    Schenk T; Franz V; Bruno N
    Vision Res; 2011 Apr; 51(8):812-8. PubMed ID: 21310170
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 2. When is grasping affected by the Müller-Lyer illusion? A quantitative review.
    Bruno N; Franz VH
    Neuropsychologia; 2009 May; 47(6):1421-33. PubMed ID: 19059422
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 3. Action without perception in human vision.
    Goodale MA
    Cogn Neuropsychol; 2008; 25(7-8):891-919. PubMed ID: 18608333
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 4. Grasping visual illusions: consistent data and no dissociation.
    Franz VH; Gegenfurtner KR
    Cogn Neuropsychol; 2008; 25(7-8):920-50. PubMed ID: 19378412
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 5. A metanalysis of the effect of the Müller-Lyer illusion on saccadic eye movements: no general support for a dissociation of perception and oculomotor action.
    Bruno N; Knox PC; de Grave DD
    Vision Res; 2010 Dec; 50(24):2671-82. PubMed ID: 20858510
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 6. The Uznadze illusion reveals similar effects of relative size on perception and action.
    Uccelli S; Pisu V; Riggio L; Bruno N
    Exp Brain Res; 2019 Apr; 237(4):953-965. PubMed ID: 30683958
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 7. Converging evidence for diverging pathways: neuropsychology and psychophysics tell the same story.
    Westwood DA; Goodale MA
    Vision Res; 2011 Apr; 51(8):804-11. PubMed ID: 20951156
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 8. An evolving view of duplex vision: separate but interacting cortical pathways for perception and action.
    Goodale MA; Westwood DA
    Curr Opin Neurobiol; 2004 Apr; 14(2):203-11. PubMed ID: 15082326
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 9. Another look at the Müller-Lyer illusion: different gaze patterns in vision for action and perception.
    van Doorn H; van der Kamp J; de Wit M; Savelsbergh GJ
    Neuropsychologia; 2009 Feb; 47(3):804-12. PubMed ID: 19118565
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 10. Consistency in exchange for inappropriately matched visual feedback? A comment on Franz and Gegenfurtner (2008) "Grasping visual illusions: consistent data and no dissociation".
    Stöttinger E; Perner J
    Cogn Neuropsychol; 2009 Jun; 26(4):412-7. PubMed ID: 19672734
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 11. [Stimulus size and visual illusion of extent].
    Bulatov A; Bertulis A; Gutauskas A; Bulatova N
    Fiziol Cheloveka; 2010; 36(2):50-8. PubMed ID: 20432691
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 12. Hemispheric specialization for the visual control of action is independent of handedness.
    Gonzalez CL; Ganel T; Goodale MA
    J Neurophysiol; 2006 Jun; 95(6):3496-501. PubMed ID: 16495359
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 13. No double-dissociation between optic ataxia and visual agnosia: multiple sub-streams for multiple visuo-manual integrations.
    Pisella L; Binkofski F; Lasek K; Toni I; Rossetti Y
    Neuropsychologia; 2006; 44(13):2734-48. PubMed ID: 16753188
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 14. Unifying sequential effects in perceptual grouping.
    Bruno N
    Trends Cogn Sci; 2005 Jan; 9(1):1-3. PubMed ID: 15639432
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 15. Knowledge in perception and illusion.
    Gregory RL
    Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci; 1997 Aug; 352(1358):1121-7. PubMed ID: 9304679
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 16. Grasping visual illusions: no evidence for a dissociation between perception and action.
    Franz VH; Gegenfurtner KR; Bülthoff HH; Fahle M
    Psychol Sci; 2000 Jan; 11(1):20-5. PubMed ID: 11228838
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 17. Dissociation of perception and action unmasked by the hollow-face illusion.
    Króliczak G; Heard P; Goodale MA; Gregory RL
    Brain Res; 2006 Mar; 1080(1):9-16. PubMed ID: 16516866
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 18. Visual illusions, delayed grasping, and memory: no shift from dorsal to ventral control.
    Franz VH; Hesse C; Kollath S
    Neuropsychologia; 2009 May; 47(6):1518-31. PubMed ID: 18834894
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 19. Dual routes to action: contributions of the dorsal and ventral streams to adaptive behavior.
    Goodale MA; Króliczak G; Westwood DA
    Prog Brain Res; 2005; 149():269-83. PubMed ID: 16226590
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 20. The Poggendorff illusion explained by natural scene geometry.
    Howe CQ; Yang Z; Purves D
    Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A; 2005 May; 102(21):7707-12. PubMed ID: 15888555
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Next]    [New Search]
    of 6.