BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

141 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 21435638)

  • 1. The intention-to-CAUSE bias: evidence from children's causal language.
    Muentener P; Lakusta L
    Cognition; 2011 Jun; 119(3):341-55. PubMed ID: 21435638
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 2. Does Making Something Move Matter? Representations of Goals and Sources in Motion Events With Causal Sources.
    Lakusta L; Muentener P; Petrillo L; Mullanaphy N; Muniz L
    Cogn Sci; 2017 Apr; 41(3):814-826. PubMed ID: 27245931
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 3. The interaction of language and thought in children's language acquisition: a crosslinguistic study.
    Weist RM; Lyytinen P; Wysocka J; Atanassova M
    J Child Lang; 1997 Feb; 24(1):81-121. PubMed ID: 9154010
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 4. Children's questions: a mechanism for cognitive development.
    Chouinard MM
    Monogr Soc Res Child Dev; 2007; 72(1):vii-ix, 1-112; discussion 113-26. PubMed ID: 17394580
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 5. Causal learning from probabilistic events in 24-month-olds: an action measure.
    Waismeyer A; Meltzoff AN; Gopnik A
    Dev Sci; 2015 Jan; 18(1):175-82. PubMed ID: 25041264
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 6. The effect of perceptual similarity and linguistic input on children's acquisition of object labels.
    Diesendruck G; Shatz M
    J Child Lang; 1997 Oct; 24(3):695-717. PubMed ID: 9519591
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 7. What, when, and how about why: a longitudinal study of early expressions of causality.
    Hood L; Bloom L
    Monogr Soc Res Child Dev; 1979; 44(6):1-47. PubMed ID: 542205
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 8. The role of abstract syntactic knowledge in language acquisition: a reply to Tomasello (2000).
    Fisher C
    Cognition; 2002 Jan; 82(3):259-78. PubMed ID: 11747864
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 9. Language and memory for motion events: origins of the asymmetry between source and goal paths.
    Lakusta L; Landau B
    Cogn Sci; 2012 Apr; 36(3):517-44. PubMed ID: 22257087
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 10. Fishy fishes: the typicality of object stimuli used to assess children's language in the Reynell Development Language Scales--III.
    Syrnyk C; Meints K
    Int J Lang Commun Disord; 2012; 47(4):437-50. PubMed ID: 22788229
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 11. Evidentiality in language and cognition.
    Papafragou A; Li P; Choi Y; Han CH
    Cognition; 2007 May; 103(2):253-99. PubMed ID: 16707120
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 12. The development of children's understanding of "because" and "so".
    French LA
    J Exp Child Psychol; 1988 Apr; 45(2):262-79. PubMed ID: 3361270
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 13. Maternal input to children with specific language impairment during shared book reading: is mothers' language in tune with their children's production?
    Majorano M; Lavelli M
    Int J Lang Commun Disord; 2014; 49(2):204-14. PubMed ID: 24224893
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 14. Influence of causal language on causal understanding: A comparison between Swiss German and Turkish.
    Ger E; Stuber L; Küntay AC; Göksun T; Stoll S; Daum MM
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2021 Oct; 210():105182. PubMed ID: 34166992
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 15. Can a microwave heat up coffee? How English- and Japanese-speaking children choose subjects in lexical causative sentences.
    Kanero J; Hirsh-Pasek K; Michnick Golinkoff R
    J Child Lang; 2016 Sep; 43(5):993-1019. PubMed ID: 26174162
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 16. The Amelia Bedelia effect: world knowledge and the goal bias in language acquisition.
    Srinivasan M; Barner D
    Cognition; 2013 Sep; 128(3):431-50. PubMed ID: 23787375
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 17. What does children's spatial language reveal about spatial concepts? Evidence from the use of containment expressions.
    Johanson M; Papafragou A
    Cogn Sci; 2014 Jun; 38(5):881-910. PubMed ID: 24641514
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 18. The acquisition and use of relative clauses in Turkish-learning children's conversational interactions: a cross-linguistic approach.
    Uzundag BA; Küntay AC
    J Child Lang; 2019 Nov; 46(6):1142-1168. PubMed ID: 31495350
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 19. Language supports young children's use of spatial relations to remember locations.
    Miller HE; Patterson R; Simmering VR
    Cognition; 2016 May; 150():170-80. PubMed ID: 26896902
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 20. Piecing together numerical language: children's use of default units in early counting and quantification.
    Brooks N; Pogue A; Barner D
    Dev Sci; 2011 Jan; 14(1):44-57. PubMed ID: 21159087
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Next]    [New Search]
    of 8.