These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

141 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 21509592)

  • 1. Neither by global nor local cues alone: evidence for a unified orientation process.
    Bodily KD; Eastman CK; Sturz BR
    Anim Cogn; 2011 Sep; 14(5):665-74. PubMed ID: 21509592
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 2. Enclosure size and the use of local and global geometric cues for reorientation.
    Sturz BR; Forloines MR; Bodily KD
    Psychon Bull Rev; 2012 Apr; 19(2):270-6. PubMed ID: 22218783
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 3. Beacons and surface features differentially influence human reliance on global and local geometric cues when reorienting in a virtual environment.
    Bodily KD; Kilday ZA; Eastman CK; Gaskin KA; Graves AA; Roberts JE; Sturz BR
    Behav Processes; 2013 Feb; 93():71-81. PubMed ID: 23089385
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 4. Orientation in trapezoid-shaped enclosures: implications for theoretical accounts of geometry learning.
    Sturz BR; Gurley T; Bodily KD
    J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process; 2011 Apr; 37(2):246-53. PubMed ID: 21319918
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 5. Does constraining field of view prevent extraction of geometric cues for humans during virtual-environment reorientation?
    Sturz BR; Kilday ZA; Bodily KD
    J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process; 2013 Oct; 39(4):390-6. PubMed ID: 23815384
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 6. Influence of distal and proximal cues in encoding geometric information.
    Vargas JP; Quintero E; López JC
    Anim Cogn; 2011 May; 14(3):351-8. PubMed ID: 21184122
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 7. Environmental scaling influences the use of local but not global geometric cues during spatial reorientation.
    Sturz BR; Bell ZK; Bodily KD
    J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn; 2018 Jul; 44(7):1159-1166. PubMed ID: 29239623
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 8. Separate geometric and non-geometric modules for spatial reorientation: evidence from a lopsided animal brain.
    Vallortigara G; Pagni P; Sovrano VA
    J Cogn Neurosci; 2004 Apr; 16(3):390-400. PubMed ID: 15072675
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 9. Reorientation in diamond-shaped environments: encoding of features and angles in enclosures versus arrays by adult humans and pigeons (Columbia livia).
    Lubyk DM; Spetch ML; Zhou R; Pisklak J; Mou W
    Anim Cogn; 2013 Jul; 16(4):565-81. PubMed ID: 23299225
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 10. Testing principal- versus medial-axis accounts of global spatial reorientation.
    Bodily KD; Sullens DG; Price SJ; Sturz BR
    J Exp Psychol Anim Learn Cogn; 2018 Apr; 44(2):209-215. PubMed ID: 29461069
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 11. Rats' use of geometric, featural and orientation cues to locate a hidden goal.
    Batty ER; Hoban L; Spetch ML; Dickson CT
    Behav Processes; 2009 Nov; 82(3):327-34. PubMed ID: 19683037
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 12. Is there an innate geometric module? Effects of experience with angular geometric cues on spatial re-orientation based on the shape of the environment.
    Chiandetti C; Vallortigara G
    Anim Cogn; 2008 Jan; 11(1):139-46. PubMed ID: 17629754
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 13. Use of local and global geometry from object arrays by adult humans.
    Reichert JF; Kelly DM
    Behav Processes; 2011 Feb; 86(2):196-205. PubMed ID: 21144887
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 14. Coding location in enclosed spaces: is geometry the principle?
    Huttenlocher J; Lourenco SF
    Dev Sci; 2007 Nov; 10(6):741-6. PubMed ID: 17973790
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 15. Geometric cues, reference frames, and the equivalence of experienced-aligned and novel-aligned views in human spatial memory.
    Kelly JW; Sjolund LA; Sturz BR
    Cognition; 2013 Mar; 126(3):459-74. PubMed ID: 23305700
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 16. More than a feeling: incidental learning of array geometry by blindfolded adult humans revealed through touch.
    Sturz BR; Green ML; Gaskin KA; Evans AC; Graves AA; Roberts JE
    J Exp Biol; 2013 Feb; 216(Pt 4):587-93. PubMed ID: 23125340
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 17. Finding the best angle: pigeons (Columba livia) weight angular information more heavily than relative wall length in an open-field geometry task.
    Lubyk DM; Spetch ML
    Anim Cogn; 2012 May; 15(3):305-12. PubMed ID: 21918871
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 18. Reorientation in a two-dimensional environment: I. Do adults encode the featural and geometric properties of a two-dimensional schematic of a room?
    Kelly DM; Spetch ML
    J Comp Psychol; 2004 Mar; 118(1):82-94. PubMed ID: 15008676
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 19. Reorienting when cues conflict: A role for information content in spatial learning?
    Sturz BR; Diemer SM
    Behav Processes; 2010 Jan; 83(1):90-8. PubMed ID: 19903512
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 20. Representation of two geometric features of the environment in the domestic chick ( Gallus gallus).
    Tommasi L; Polli C
    Anim Cogn; 2004 Jan; 7(1):53-9. PubMed ID: 12884079
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Next]    [New Search]
    of 8.