These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.
228 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 21557743)
1. Structure mapping and relational language support children's learning of relational categories. Gentner D; Anggoro FK; Klibanoff RS Child Dev; 2011; 82(4):1173-88. PubMed ID: 21557743 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
2. Relational language and the development of relational mapping. Loewenstein J; Gentner D Cogn Psychol; 2005 Jun; 50(4):315-53. PubMed ID: 15893523 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
3. Focusing on the relation: fewer exemplars facilitate children's initial verb learning and extension. Maguire MJ; Hirsh-Pasek K; Golinkoff RM; Brandone AC Dev Sci; 2008 Jul; 11(4):628-34. PubMed ID: 18576970 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
4. How do parents respond to children's questions about the identity of artifacts? Nelson DG; O'Neil K Dev Sci; 2005 Nov; 8(6):519-24. PubMed ID: 16246243 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
6. Children's inductive inferences within superordinate categories: the role of language and category structure. Gelman SA; O'Reilly AW Child Dev; 1988 Aug; 59(4):876-87. PubMed ID: 3168626 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
9. Cross-classification and category representation in children's concepts. Nguyen SP Dev Psychol; 2007 May; 43(3):719-31. PubMed ID: 17484583 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
10. Young children's inductions from natural kinds: the role of categories and appearances. Gelman SA; Markman EM Child Dev; 1987 Dec; 58(6):1532-41. PubMed ID: 3691200 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
11. A cross-linguistic examination of the noun-category bias: its existence and specificity in French- and Spanish-speaking preschool-aged children. Waxman SR; Senghas A; Benveniste S Cogn Psychol; 1997 Apr; 32(3):183-218. PubMed ID: 9125905 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
12. Incorporating new words into the lexicon: preliminary evidence for language hierarchies in two-year-old children. Taylor M; Gelman SA Child Dev; 1989 Jun; 60(3):625-36. PubMed ID: 2737012 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
13. Beyond labeling: the role of maternal input in the acquisition of richly structured categories. Gelman SA; Coley JD; Rosengren KS; Hartman E; Pappas A Monogr Soc Res Child Dev; 1998; 63(1):I-V, 1-148; discussion 149-57. PubMed ID: 9788141 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
14. Information learned from generic language becomes central to children's biological concepts: evidence from their open-ended explanations. Cimpian A; Markman EM Cognition; 2009 Oct; 113(1):14-25. PubMed ID: 19674739 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
15. Acquiring color names via linguistic contrast: the influence of contrasting terms. Au TK; Laframboise DE Child Dev; 1990 Dec; 61(6):1808-23. PubMed ID: 2083500 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
16. Expressing generic concepts with and without a language model. Goldin-Meadow S; Gelman SA; Mylander C Cognition; 2005 Jun; 96(2):109-26. PubMed ID: 15925572 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
17. Compositionality and statistics in adjective acquisition: 4-year-olds interpret tall and short based on the size distributions of novel noun referents. Barner D; Snedeker J Child Dev; 2008; 79(3):594-608. PubMed ID: 18489415 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
18. The logical syntax of number words: theory, acquisition and processing. Musolino J Cognition; 2009 Apr; 111(1):24-45. PubMed ID: 19217616 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
19. The empirical case for role-governed categories. Goldwater MB; Markman AB; Stilwell CH Cognition; 2011 Mar; 118(3):359-76. PubMed ID: 21147480 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
20. The effects of theories on children's acquisition of family-resemblance categories. Krascum RM; Andrews S Child Dev; 1998 Apr; 69(2):333-46. PubMed ID: 9586209 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related] [Next] [New Search]