These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.
473 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 21624567)
1. Lifespan aging and belief reasoning: Influences of executive function and social cue decoding. Phillips LH; Bull R; Allen R; Insch P; Burr K; Ogg W Cognition; 2011 Aug; 120(2):236-47. PubMed ID: 21624567 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
2. Developmental continuity in theory of mind: speed and accuracy of belief-desire reasoning in children and adults. Apperly IA; Warren F; Andrews BJ; Grant J; Todd S Child Dev; 2011; 82(5):1691-703. PubMed ID: 21883156 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
3. Theory of mind through the ages: older and middle-aged adults exhibit more errors than do younger adults on a continuous false belief task. Bernstein DM; Thornton WL; Sommerville JA Exp Aging Res; 2011 Oct; 37(5):481-502. PubMed ID: 22091578 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
4. Domain-general contributions to social reasoning: theory of mind and deontic reasoning re-explored. McKinnon MC; Moscovitch M Cognition; 2007 Feb; 102(2):179-218. PubMed ID: 16480974 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
5. Making sense of theory of mind and paranoia: the psychometric properties and reasoning requirements of a false belief sequencing task. Corcoran R; Bentall RP; Rowse G; Moore R; Cummins S; Blackwood N; Howard R; Shryane NM Cogn Neuropsychiatry; 2011 Nov; 16(6):547-61. PubMed ID: 21838640 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
6. Age effects on different components of theory of mind. Duval C; Piolino P; Bejanin A; Eustache F; Desgranges B Conscious Cogn; 2011 Sep; 20(3):627-42. PubMed ID: 21111637 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
7. The decline of theory of mind in old age is (partly) mediated by developmental changes in domain-general abilities. Rakoczy H; Harder-Kasten A; Sturm L Br J Psychol; 2012 Feb; 103(1):58-72. PubMed ID: 22229774 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
8. Modulation of the cortical false belief network during development. Sommer M; Meinhardt J; Eichenmüller K; Sodian B; Döhnel K; Hajak G Brain Res; 2010 Oct; 1354():123-31. PubMed ID: 20678489 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
9. Exploring the specificity of age-related differences in theory of mind tasks. Slessor G; Phillips LH; Bull R Psychol Aging; 2007 Sep; 22(3):639-43. PubMed ID: 17874961 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
10. Effects of aging on hemispheric asymmetry in inferior frontal cortex activity during belief-bias syllogistic reasoning: a near-infrared spectroscopy study. Tsujii T; Okada M; Watanabe S Behav Brain Res; 2010 Jul; 210(2):178-83. PubMed ID: 20171989 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
11. Reasoning about other people's beliefs: bilinguals have an advantage. Rubio-Fernández P; Glucksberg S J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn; 2012 Jan; 38(1):211-7. PubMed ID: 21875251 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
12. Perceptual Access Reasoning (PAR) in Developing a Representational Theory of Mind. Fabricius WV; Gonzales CR; Pesch A; Weimer AA; Pugliese J; Carroll K; Bolnick RR; Kupfer AS; Eisenberg N; Spinrad TL Monogr Soc Res Child Dev; 2021 Sep; 86(3):7-154. PubMed ID: 34580875 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
13. Functional activity of the right temporo-parietal junction and of the medial prefrontal cortex associated with true and false belief reasoning. Döhnel K; Schuwerk T; Meinhardt J; Sodian B; Hajak G; Sommer M Neuroimage; 2012 Apr; 60(3):1652-61. PubMed ID: 22300812 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
14. Beyond false belief: theory of mind in young, young-old, and old-old adults. Cavallini E; Lecce S; Bottiroli S; Palladino P; Pagnin A Int J Aging Hum Dev; 2013; 76(3):181-98. PubMed ID: 23781702 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
15. Representational and executive selection resources in 'theory of mind': evidence from compromised belief-desire reasoning in old age. German TP; Hehman JA Cognition; 2006 Aug; 101(1):129-52. PubMed ID: 16288734 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
16. Individual differences in aging and cognitive control modulate the neural indexes of context updating and maintenance during task switching. Adrover-Roig D; Barceló F Cortex; 2010 Apr; 46(4):434-50. PubMed ID: 19889406 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
17. Frontal and temporo-parietal lobe contributions to theory of mind: neuropsychological evidence from a false-belief task with reduced language and executive demands. Apperly IA; Samson D; Chiavarino C; Humphreys GW J Cogn Neurosci; 2004 Dec; 16(10):1773-84. PubMed ID: 15701227 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
18. Implicit false belief tracking is preserved in late adulthood. Grainger SA; Henry JD; Naughtin CK; Comino MS; Dux PE Q J Exp Psychol (Hove); 2018 Sep; 71(9):1980-1987. PubMed ID: 30117384 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
19. Theory of mind deficits in Parkinson's disease: a product of executive dysfunction? Eddy CM; Beck SR; Mitchell IJ; Praamstra P; Pall HS Neuropsychology; 2013 Jan; 27(1):37-47. PubMed ID: 23356595 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
20. [Influence of attention on an auditory-verbal learning test in schizophrenic patients]. Huguelet P; Nicastro R; Zanello A Encephale; 2002; 28(4):291-7. PubMed ID: 12232538 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related] [Next] [New Search]