These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.
167 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 21771614)
1. Simple gaze analysis and special design of a virtual Morris water maze provides a new method for differentiating egocentric and allocentric navigational strategy choice. Livingstone-Lee SA; Murchison S; Zeman PM; Gandhi M; van Gerven D; Stewart L; Livingston NJ; Skelton RW Behav Brain Res; 2011 Nov; 225(1):117-25. PubMed ID: 21771614 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
2. Developmental time course of the acquisition of sequential egocentric and allocentric navigation strategies. Bullens J; Iglói K; Berthoz A; Postma A; Rondi-Reig L J Exp Child Psychol; 2010 Nov; 107(3):337-50. PubMed ID: 20598705 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
3. Spontaneous navigational strategies and performance in the virtual town. Etchamendy N; Bohbot VD Hippocampus; 2007; 17(8):595-9. PubMed ID: 17546682 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
4. Virtual environment navigation tasks and the assessment of cognitive deficits in individuals with brain injury. Livingstone SA; Skelton RW Behav Brain Res; 2007 Dec; 185(1):21-31. PubMed ID: 17727970 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
6. Direct measurement of spontaneous strategy selection in a virtual Morris water maze shows females choose an allocentric strategy at least as often as males do. van Gerven DJ; Schneider AN; Wuitchik DM; Skelton RW Behav Neurosci; 2012 Jun; 126(3):465-78. PubMed ID: 22642888 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
7. Sex differences in a virtual water maze: an eye tracking and pupillometry study. Mueller SC; Jackson CP; Skelton RW Behav Brain Res; 2008 Nov; 193(2):209-15. PubMed ID: 18602173 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
8. Human navigation that requires calculating heading vectors recruits parietal cortex in a virtual and visually sparse water maze task in fMRI. Rodriguez PF Behav Neurosci; 2010 Aug; 124(4):532-40. PubMed ID: 20695652 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
9. The effects of overtraining in the Morris water maze on allocentric and egocentric learning strategies in rats. Kealy J; Diviney M; Kehoe E; McGonagle V; O'Shea A; Harvey D; Commins S Behav Brain Res; 2008 Oct; 192(2):259-63. PubMed ID: 18514924 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
10. Spatial deficits in a virtual water maze in amnesic participants with hippocampal damage. Goodrich-Hunsaker NJ; Livingstone SA; Skelton RW; Hopkins RO Hippocampus; 2010 Apr; 20(4):481-91. PubMed ID: 19554566 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
11. Altered navigational strategy use and visuospatial deficits in hAPP transgenic mice. Deipolyi AR; Fang S; Palop JJ; Yu GQ; Wang X; Mucke L Neurobiol Aging; 2008 Feb; 29(2):253-66. PubMed ID: 17126954 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
12. Sex differences in human virtual water maze performance: novel measures reveal the relative contribution of directional responding and spatial knowledge. Woolley DG; Vermaercke B; Op de Beeck H; Wagemans J; Gantois I; D'Hooge R; Swinnen SP; Wenderoth N Behav Brain Res; 2010 Apr; 208(2):408-14. PubMed ID: 20035800 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
13. Hex Maze: A new virtual maze able to track acquisition and usage of three navigation strategies. Spriggs MJ; Kirk IJ; Skelton RW Behav Brain Res; 2018 Feb; 339():195-206. PubMed ID: 29203335 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
14. Control of rodent and human spatial navigation by room and apparatus cues. Hamilton DA; Johnson TE; Redhead ES; Verney SP Behav Processes; 2009 Jun; 81(2):154-69. PubMed ID: 19121374 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
15. Emergence of an egocentric cue guiding and allocentric inferring strategy that mirrors hippocampal brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) expression in the Morris water maze. Harvey DR; McGauran AM; Murphy J; Burns L; McMonagle E; Commins S Neurobiol Learn Mem; 2008 May; 89(4):462-79. PubMed ID: 17923424 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
16. Allocentric versus egocentric spatial memory after unilateral temporal lobectomy in humans. Feigenbaum JD; Morris RG Neuropsychology; 2004 Jul; 18(3):462-72. PubMed ID: 15291724 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
17. The human parahippocampal cortex subserves egocentric spatial learning during navigation in a virtual maze. Weniger G; Siemerkus J; Schmidt-Samoa C; Mehlitz M; Baudewig J; Dechent P; Irle E Neurobiol Learn Mem; 2010 Jan; 93(1):46-55. PubMed ID: 19683063 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
18. Sequential egocentric strategy is acquired as early as allocentric strategy: Parallel acquisition of these two navigation strategies. Iglói K; Zaoui M; Berthoz A; Rondi-Reig L Hippocampus; 2009 Dec; 19(12):1199-211. PubMed ID: 19360853 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
19. Sex differences in a landmark environmental re-orientation task only during the learning phase. Piccardi L; Bianchini F; Iasevoli L; Giannone G; Guariglia C Neurosci Lett; 2011 Oct; 503(3):181-5. PubMed ID: 21889578 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
20. Exploration of virtual mazes by rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta). Washburn DA; Astur RS Anim Cogn; 2003 Sep; 6(3):161-8. PubMed ID: 12750961 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related] [Next] [New Search]