These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

167 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 21771614)

  • 21. Design of a Virtual Reality Navigational (VRN) experiment for assessment of egocentric spatial cognition.
    Byagowi A; Moussavi Z
    Annu Int Conf IEEE Eng Med Biol Soc; 2012; 2012():4812-5. PubMed ID: 23367004
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 22. Pictorial gaze cues do not enhance long-tailed macaques' performance on a computerised object-location task.
    Vick SJ; Toxopeus I; Anderson JR
    Behav Processes; 2006 Nov; 73(3):308-14. PubMed ID: 16962253
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 23. Acute stress switches spatial navigation strategy from egocentric to allocentric in a virtual Morris water maze.
    van Gerven DJH; Ferguson T; Skelton RW
    Neurobiol Learn Mem; 2016 Jul; 132():29-39. PubMed ID: 27174311
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 24. Egocentric and allocentric memory as assessed by virtual reality in individuals with amnestic mild cognitive impairment.
    Weniger G; Ruhleder M; Lange C; Wolf S; Irle E
    Neuropsychologia; 2011 Feb; 49(3):518-27. PubMed ID: 21185847
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 25. Virtual navigation for memory rehabilitation in a traumatic brain injured patient.
    Caglio M; Latini-Corazzini L; D'Agata F; Cauda F; Sacco K; Monteverdi S; Zettin M; Duca S; Geminiani G
    Neurocase; 2012; 18(2):123-31. PubMed ID: 22352998
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 26. Eye-movements during navigation in a virtual tunnel.
    Gramann K; El Sharkawy J; Deubel H
    Int J Neurosci; 2009; 119(10):1755-78. PubMed ID: 19922385
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 27. Incidental learning of allocentric and egocentric strategies by both men and women in a dual-strategy virtual Morris Water Maze.
    Ferguson TD; Livingstone-Lee SA; Skelton RW
    Behav Brain Res; 2019 May; 364():281-295. PubMed ID: 30794853
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 28. Egocentric memory impaired and allocentric memory intact as assessed by virtual reality in subjects with unilateral parietal cortex lesions.
    Weniger G; Ruhleder M; Wolf S; Lange C; Irle E
    Neuropsychologia; 2009 Jan; 47(1):59-69. PubMed ID: 18789955
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 29. Design and application of real-time visual attention model for the exploration of 3D virtual environments.
    Hillaire S; Lécuyer A; Regia-Corte T; Cozot R; Royan J; Breton G
    IEEE Trans Vis Comput Graph; 2012 Mar; 18(3):356-68. PubMed ID: 21931178
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 30. Orientation of lizards in a Morris water-maze: roles of the sun compass and the parietal eye.
    Foà A; Basaglia F; Beltrami G; Carnacina M; Moretto E; Bertolucci C
    J Exp Biol; 2009 Sep; 212(18):2918-24. PubMed ID: 19717673
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 31. Effects of age on virtual environment place navigation and allocentric cognitive mapping.
    Moffat SD; Resnick SM
    Behav Neurosci; 2002 Oct; 116(5):851-9. PubMed ID: 12369805
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 32. Hippocampal function and spatial memory: evidence from functional neuroimaging in healthy participants and performance of patients with medial temporal lobe resections.
    Bohbot VD; Iaria G; Petrides M
    Neuropsychology; 2004 Jul; 18(3):418-25. PubMed ID: 15291720
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 33. Learning associations between places and visual cues without learning to navigate: neither fornix nor entorhinal cortex is required.
    Gaffan EA; Bannerman DM; Healey AN
    Hippocampus; 2003; 13(4):445-60. PubMed ID: 12836914
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 34. Modeling the interaction of navigational systems in a reward-based virtual navigation task.
    Raiesdana S
    J Integr Neurosci; 2018; 17(1):27-42. PubMed ID: 29376881
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 35. Use of salient and non-salient visuospatial cues by rats in the Morris Water Maze.
    Young GS; Choleris E; Kirkland JB
    Physiol Behav; 2006 Apr; 87(4):794-9. PubMed ID: 16516936
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 36. Men and women differ in object memory but not performance of a virtual radial maze.
    Levy LJ; Astur RS; Frick KM
    Behav Neurosci; 2005 Aug; 119(4):853-62. PubMed ID: 16187814
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 37. Spatial radial maze procedures and setups to dissociate local and distal relational spatial frameworks in humans.
    Bertholet L; Escobar MT; Depré M; Chavan CF; Giuliani F; Gisquet-Verrier P; Preissmann D; Schenk F
    J Neurosci Methods; 2015 Sep; 253():126-41. PubMed ID: 26096715
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 38. Navigating in a virtual three-dimensional maze: how do egocentric and allocentric reference frames interact?
    Vidal M; Amorim MA; Berthoz A
    Brain Res Cogn Brain Res; 2004 May; 19(3):244-58. PubMed ID: 15062862
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 39. Evidence for the influence of testosterone in the performance of spatial navigation in a virtual water maze in women but not in men.
    Burkitt J; Widman D; Saucier DM
    Horm Behav; 2007 May; 51(5):649-54. PubMed ID: 17462646
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 40. Representational neglect and navigation in real space.
    Guariglia C; Piccardi L; Iaria G; Nico D; Pizzamiglio L
    Neuropsychologia; 2005; 43(8):1138-43. PubMed ID: 15817171
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Previous]   [Next]    [New Search]
    of 9.