These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.
3. Preschoolers continue to trust a more accurate informant 1 week after exposure to accuracy information. Corriveau K; Harris PL Dev Sci; 2009 Jan; 12(1):188-93. PubMed ID: 19120427 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
4. Preschoolers monitor the relative accuracy of informants. Pasquini ES; Corriveau KH; Koenig M; Harris PL Dev Psychol; 2007 Sep; 43(5):1216-26. PubMed ID: 17723046 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
5. Choosing your informant: weighing familiarity and recent accuracy. Corriveau K; Harris PL Dev Sci; 2009 Apr; 12(3):426-37. PubMed ID: 19371367 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
6. Trust in testimony about strangers: young children prefer reliable informants who make positive attributions. Boseovski JJ J Exp Child Psychol; 2012 Mar; 111(3):543-51. PubMed ID: 22115450 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
7. Going with the flow: preschoolers prefer nondissenters as informants. Corriveau KH; Fusaro M; Harris PL Psychol Sci; 2009 Mar; 20(3):372-7. PubMed ID: 19207691 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
8. When being right is not enough: four-year-olds distinguish knowledgeable informants from merely accurate informants. Einav S; Robinson EJ Psychol Sci; 2011 Oct; 22(10):1250-3. PubMed ID: 21881060 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
9. Five-year-olds are willing, but 4-year-olds refuse, to trust informants who offer new and unfamiliar labels for parts of the body. Luu B; Rosnay Md; Harris PL J Exp Child Psychol; 2013 Oct; 116(2):234-46. PubMed ID: 23872524 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
10. Developmental differences in the relative weighing of informants' social attributes. Reyes-Jaquez B; Echols CH Dev Psychol; 2013 Mar; 49(3):602-13. PubMed ID: 23356527 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
11. Accuracy trumps accent in children's endorsement of object labels. Corriveau KH; Kinzler KD; Harris PL Dev Psychol; 2013 Mar; 49(3):470-9. PubMed ID: 23231692 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
12. Children assess informant reliability using bystanders' non-verbal cues. Fusaro M; Harris PL Dev Sci; 2008 Sep; 11(5):771-7. PubMed ID: 18801133 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
14. Preschoolers trust particular informants when learning new names and new morphological forms. Corriveau KH; Pickard K; Harris PL Br J Dev Psychol; 2011 Mar; 29(Pt 1):46-63. PubMed ID: 21199505 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
15. Generic language and speaker confidence guide preschoolers' inferences about novel animate kinds. Stock HR; Graham SA; Chambers CG Dev Psychol; 2009 May; 45(3):884-8. PubMed ID: 19413440 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
16. "Who can help me fix this toy?" The distinction between causal knowledge and word knowledge guides preschoolers' selective requests for information. Kushnir T; Vredenburgh C; Schneider LA Dev Psychol; 2013 Mar; 49(3):446-53. PubMed ID: 23339590 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
17. 'I bet you know more and are nicer too!': what children infer from others' accuracy. Brosseau-Liard PE; Birch SA Dev Sci; 2010 Sep; 13(5):772-8. PubMed ID: 20712743 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
18. The role of trait reasoning in young children's selective trust. Hermes J; Behne T; Rakoczy H Dev Psychol; 2015 Nov; 51(11):1574-87. PubMed ID: 26389602 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
20. Do children trust based on group membership or prior accuracy? The role of novel group membership in children's trust decisions. Elashi FB; Mills CM J Exp Child Psychol; 2014 Dec; 128():88-104. PubMed ID: 25108696 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related] [Next] [New Search]