These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

185 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 21824130)

  • 1. The development of distrust.
    Vanderbilt KE; Liu D; Heyman GD
    Child Dev; 2011; 82(5):1372-80. PubMed ID: 21824130
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 2. Selective trust: children's use of intention and outcome of past testimony.
    Liu D; Vanderbilt KE; Heyman GD
    Dev Psychol; 2013 Mar; 49(3):439-45. PubMed ID: 23339589
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 3. Young children's trust in overtly misleading advice.
    Heyman GD; Sritanyaratana L; Vanderbilt KE
    Cogn Sci; 2013; 37(4):646-67. PubMed ID: 23294130
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 4. Young children's use of honesty as a basis for selective trust.
    Li QG; Heyman GD; Xu F; Lee K
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2014 Jan; 117():59-72. PubMed ID: 24149377
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 5. Detecting lies through others' eyes: Children use perceptual access cues to evaluate listeners' beliefs about informants' deception.
    Tay C; Ng R; Ye NN; Ding XP
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2024 May; 241():105863. PubMed ID: 38306738
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 6. In the absence of conflicting testimony young children trust inaccurate informants.
    Vanderbilt KE; Heyman GD; Liu D
    Dev Sci; 2014 May; 17(3):443-51. PubMed ID: 24444426
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 7. In the presence and absence of conflicting testimony, children's selective trust in the in-group informant in moral judgment and knowledge access.
    Yang R; Zhang L; Wu X
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2023 Jul; 231():105664. PubMed ID: 36913792
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 8. The role of epistemic and social characteristics in children's selective trust: Three meta-analyses.
    Tong Y; Wang F; Danovitch J
    Dev Sci; 2020 Mar; 23(2):e12895. PubMed ID: 31433880
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 9. Epistemic trust: modeling children's reasoning about others' knowledge and intent.
    Shafto P; Eaves B; Navarro DJ; Perfors A
    Dev Sci; 2012 May; 15(3):436-47. PubMed ID: 22490183
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 10. Trust me, I'm a competent expert: Developmental differences in children's use of an expert's explanation quality to infer trustworthiness.
    Clegg JM; Kurkul KE; Corriveau KH
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2019 Dec; 188():104670. PubMed ID: 31499458
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 11. Epistemic Vigilance in Early Ontogeny: Children's Use of Nonverbal Behavior to Detect Deception.
    Ghossainy ME; Al-Shawaf L; Woolley JD
    Evol Psychol; 2021; 19(1):1474704920986860. PubMed ID: 33499655
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 12. Success inhibits preschoolers' ability to establish selective trust.
    Palmquist CM; Jaswal VK; Rutherford A
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2016 Dec; 152():192-204. PubMed ID: 27569645
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 13. Five-year-olds are willing, but 4-year-olds refuse, to trust informants who offer new and unfamiliar labels for parts of the body.
    Luu B; Rosnay Md; Harris PL
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2013 Oct; 116(2):234-46. PubMed ID: 23872524
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 14. Children's and adults' epistemic trust in and impressions of inaccurate informants.
    Ronfard S; Lane JD
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2019 Dec; 188():104662. PubMed ID: 31470226
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 15. The pragmatic role of trust in young children's interpretation of unfamiliar signals.
    Mascaro O; Sperber D
    PLoS One; 2019; 14(10):e0224648. PubMed ID: 31665195
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 16. Selective information seeking after a single encounter.
    Fitneva SA; Dunfield KA
    Dev Psychol; 2010 Sep; 46(5):1380-4. PubMed ID: 20822247
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 17. When being right is not enough: four-year-olds distinguish knowledgeable informants from merely accurate informants.
    Einav S; Robinson EJ
    Psychol Sci; 2011 Oct; 22(10):1250-3. PubMed ID: 21881060
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 18. Characterizing children's expectations about expertise and incompetence: halo or pitchfork effects?
    Koenig MA; Jaswal VK
    Child Dev; 2011; 82(5):1634-47. PubMed ID: 21790541
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 19. "Why does rain fall?": children prefer to learn from an informant who uses noncircular explanations.
    Corriveau KH; Kurkul KE
    Child Dev; 2014; 85(5):1827-35. PubMed ID: 24646210
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 20. Parameterizing developmental changes in epistemic trust.
    Eaves BS; Shafto P
    Psychon Bull Rev; 2017 Apr; 24(2):277-306. PubMed ID: 27401075
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Next]    [New Search]
    of 10.