These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

119 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 21842978)

  • 1. Hamsters' (Mesocricetus auratus) memory in a radial maze analog: the role of spatial versus olfactory cues.
    Tonneau F; Cabrera F; Corujo A
    J Comp Psychol; 2012 Feb; 126(1):82-6. PubMed ID: 21842978
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 2. Spatial guidance of choice behavior in the radial-arm maze.
    Brown MF; Rish PA; VonCulin JE; Edberg JA
    J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process; 1993 Jul; 19(3):195-214. PubMed ID: 8340766
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 3. How golden hamsters (Mesocricetus auratus) discriminate top from bottom flank scents in over-marks.
    Cohen AB; Johnston RE; Kwon A
    J Comp Psychol; 2001 Sep; 115(3):241-7. PubMed ID: 11594493
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 4. Dwarf and golden hamsters' differing responses to conspecific signals for food.
    Lupfer-Johnson G
    J Gen Psychol; 2008 Jan; 135(1):54-64. PubMed ID: 18318408
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 5. Spatial learning and memory in the tortoise (Geochelone carbonaria).
    Wilkinson A; Chan HM; Hall G
    J Comp Psychol; 2007 Nov; 121(4):412-8. PubMed ID: 18085925
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 6. Spatial memory and instrumental conditioning.
    Buresová O
    Acta Neurobiol Exp (Wars); 1980; 40(1):51-65. PubMed ID: 7424593
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 7. Hamsters (Mesocricetus auratus ) use spatial memory in foraging for food to hoard.
    Jones CH; McGhee R; Wilkie DM
    Behav Processes; 1990 Jun; 21(2-3):179-87. PubMed ID: 24925848
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 8. Adaptive changes in a radial maze task: Efficient selection of baited arms with reduced foraging in senescent hooded rats.
    Grandchamp N; Schenk F
    Behav Brain Res; 2006 Mar; 168(1):161-6. PubMed ID: 16325933
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 9. Sex differences and correlations in a virtual Morris water task, a virtual radial arm maze, and mental rotation.
    Astur RS; Tropp J; Sava S; Constable RT; Markus EJ
    Behav Brain Res; 2004 May; 151(1-2):103-15. PubMed ID: 15084426
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 10. Spatial behaviour in golden hamster (Mesocricetus auratus): a returning task in a T-maze with no intra- or extra-maze cues available.
    Chapuis N
    Percept Mot Skills; 1982 Feb; 54(1):337-8. PubMed ID: 7063352
    [No Abstract]   [Full Text] [Related]  

  • 11. Flexible memory processing by rats: use of prospective and retrospective information in the radial maze.
    Cook RG; Brown MF; Riley DA
    J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process; 1985 Jul; 11(3):453-69. PubMed ID: 4009126
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 12. Remote spatial memory and the hippocampus: effect of early and extensive training in the radial maze.
    Ramos JM
    Learn Mem; 2009 Sep; 16(9):554-63. PubMed ID: 19713354
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 13. Volume discrimination learning in golden hamsters: effects of the structure of complex rearing cages.
    Thinus-Blanc C
    Dev Psychobiol; 1981 Jul; 14(4):397-403. PubMed ID: 7250528
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 14. Perception of scent over-marks by golden hamsters (Mesocricetus auratus): novel mechanisms for determining which individual's mark is on top.
    Johnston RE; Bhorade A
    J Comp Psychol; 1998 Sep; 112(3):230-43. PubMed ID: 9770313
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 15. Golden hamsters on the eight-arm maze in light and darkness: the role of dead reckoning.
    Etienne AS; Sitbon S; Dahn-Hurni C; Maurer R
    Q J Exp Psychol B; 1994 Nov; 47(4):401-25. PubMed ID: 7809405
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 16. Interaction between the nature of the information and the cognitive requirement of the task in problem solving in mice.
    Wolff M; Benhassine N; Costet P; Segu L; Buhot MC
    Brain Res Cogn Brain Res; 2004 Nov; 21(3):289-300. PubMed ID: 15511645
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 17. Retention of cue-based associations in the water maze is time-dependent and sensitive to disruption by rotating the starting position.
    McGauran AM; Harvey D; Cunningham L; Craig S; Commins S
    Behav Brain Res; 2004 May; 151(1-2):255-66. PubMed ID: 15084441
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 18. Social transmission of flavor preferences in two species of hamsters (Mesocricetus auratus and Phodopus campbelli).
    Lupfer G; Frieman J; Coonfield D
    J Comp Psychol; 2003 Dec; 117(4):449-55. PubMed ID: 14717647
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 19. Rats acquire spatial learning sets.
    Zeldin RK; Olton DS
    J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process; 1986 Oct; 12(4):412-9. PubMed ID: 3772305
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 20. Neurotoxic lesions of the rat perirhinal cortex fail to disrupt the acquisition or performance of tests of allocentric spatial memory.
    Machin P; Vann SD; Muir JL; Aggleton JP
    Behav Neurosci; 2002 Apr; 116(2):232-40. PubMed ID: 11996309
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Next]    [New Search]
    of 6.