These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

275 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 21969135)

  • 61. Substrate optimization in nut cracking by capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella).
    Pouydebat E; Gorce P; Bels V; Coppens Y
    Am J Primatol; 2006 Oct; 68(10):1017-24. PubMed ID: 16892415
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 62. Honeybee methodology, cognition, and theory: recording local directional decisions in interpatch foraging and interpreting their theoretical relevance.
    Najera DA; Jander R
    Anim Cogn; 2012 Mar; 15(2):251-63. PubMed ID: 21912853
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 63. Capuchins, space, time and memory: an experimental test of what-where-when memory in wild monkeys.
    Janson CH
    Proc Biol Sci; 2016 Oct; 283(1840):. PubMed ID: 27708145
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 64. Anointing variation across wild capuchin populations: a review of material preferences, bout frequency and anointing sociality in Cebus and Sapajus.
    Alfaro JW; Matthews L; Boyette AH; Macfarlan SJ; Phillips KA; Falótico T; Ottoni E; Verderane M; Izar P; Schulte M; Melin A; Fedigan L; Janson C; Alfaro ME
    Am J Primatol; 2012 Apr; 74(4):299-314. PubMed ID: 21769906
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 65. Partner effects on food consumption in brown capuchin monkeys.
    Dindo M; De Waal FB
    Am J Primatol; 2007 Apr; 69(4):448-56. PubMed ID: 17146793
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 66. Social context and consumption of unfamiliar foods by capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella) over repeated encounters.
    Visalberghi E; Valente M; Fragaszy D
    Am J Primatol; 1998; 45(4):367-80. PubMed ID: 9702282
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 67. Sensory information and associative cues used in food detection by wild vervet monkeys.
    Teichroeb JA; Chapman CA
    Anim Cogn; 2014 May; 17(3):517-28. PubMed ID: 24045849
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 68. Experimental field study of the relative costs and benefits to wild tamarins (Saguinus imperator and S. fuscicollis) of exploiting contestable food patches as single- and mixed-species troops.
    Bicca-Marques JC; Garber PA
    Am J Primatol; 2003 Aug; 60(4):139-53. PubMed ID: 12910465
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 69. Behavioral flexibility of a group of bearded capuchin monkeys (Cebus libidinosus) in the National Park of Brasília (Brazil): consequences of cohabitation with visitors.
    Sabbatini G; Stammati M; Tavares MC; Visalberghi E
    Braz J Biol; 2008 Nov; 68(4):685-93. PubMed ID: 19197485
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 70. Capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella) use positive, but not negative, auditory cues to infer food location.
    Heimbauer LA; Antworth RL; Owren MJ
    Anim Cogn; 2012 Jan; 15(1):45-55. PubMed ID: 21681475
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 71. A capuchin monkey (Cebus apella) uses video to find food.
    Potì P; Saporiti M
    Folia Primatol (Basel); 2010; 81(1):16-30. PubMed ID: 20357474
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 72. Capuchins (Cebus apella) can solve a means-end problem.
    Yocom AM; Boysen ST
    J Comp Psychol; 2010 Aug; 124(3):271-7. PubMed ID: 20695658
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 73. Redundant food searches by capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella): a failure of metacognition?
    Paukner A; Anderson JR; Fujita K
    Anim Cogn; 2006 Apr; 9(2):110-7. PubMed ID: 16184375
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 74. The effects of spatial food distribution and group size on foraging behaviour in a benthic fish.
    Stenberg M; Persson A
    Behav Processes; 2005 Aug; 70(1):41-50. PubMed ID: 15908140
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 75. Adaptability in stone tool use by wild capuchin monkeys (Sapajus libidinosus).
    De Moraes BL; Da Silva Souto A; Schiel N
    Am J Primatol; 2014 Oct; 76(10):967-77. PubMed ID: 24753103
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 76. Patterns of individual diet choice and efficiency of foraging in wedge-capped capuchin monkeys (Cebus olivaceus).
    Fragaszy DM; Boinski S
    J Comp Psychol; 1995 Dec; 109(4):339-348. PubMed ID: 7497693
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 77. Three stones for three seeds: natural occurrence of selective tool use by capuchins (Cebus libidinosus) based on an analysis of the weight of stones found at nutting sites.
    Ferreira RG; Emidio RA; Jerusalinsky L
    Am J Primatol; 2010 Mar; 72(3):270-5. PubMed ID: 19953558
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 78. Route and landmark learning by rats searching for food.
    Tamara C; Timberlake W
    Behav Processes; 2011 Jan; 86(1):125-32. PubMed ID: 21044664
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 79. Ontogeny of Foraging Competence in Capuchin Monkeys (Cebus capucinus) for Easy versus Difficult to Acquire Fruits: A Test of the Needing to Learn Hypothesis.
    Eadie EC
    PLoS One; 2015; 10(9):e0138001. PubMed ID: 26372642
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 80. Capuchin monkeys' use of human and conspecific cues to solve a hidden object-choice task.
    Essler JL; Schwartz LP; Rossettie MS; Judge PG
    Anim Cogn; 2017 Sep; 20(5):985-998. PubMed ID: 28741081
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Previous]   [Next]    [New Search]
    of 14.