These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

209 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 22010899)

  • 21. Seventeen-month-olds appeal to false beliefs to interpret others' referential communication.
    Southgate V; Chevallier C; Csibra G
    Dev Sci; 2010 Nov; 13(6):907-12. PubMed ID: 20977561
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 22. Can infants adopt underspecified contents into attributed beliefs? Representational prerequisites of theory of mind.
    Kovács ÁM; Téglás E; Csibra G
    Cognition; 2021 Aug; 213():104640. PubMed ID: 33757642
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 23. Do people automatically track others' beliefs? Evidence from a continuous measure.
    van der Wel RP; Sebanz N; Knoblich G
    Cognition; 2014 Jan; 130(1):128-33. PubMed ID: 24216021
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 24. Great apes anticipate that other individuals will act according to false beliefs.
    Krupenye C; Kano F; Hirata S; Call J; Tomasello M
    Science; 2016 Oct; 354(6308):110-114. PubMed ID: 27846501
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 25. Functional Organization of the Temporal-Parietal Junction for Theory of Mind in Preverbal Infants: A Near-Infrared Spectroscopy Study.
    Hyde DC; Simon CE; Ting F; Nikolaeva JI
    J Neurosci; 2018 May; 38(18):4264-4274. PubMed ID: 29593053
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 26. The evolution of social cognition: goal familiarity shapes monkeys' action understanding.
    Rochat MJ; Serra E; Fadiga L; Gallese V
    Curr Biol; 2008 Feb; 18(3):227-32. PubMed ID: 18221878
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 27. Mentalizing regions represent distributed, continuous, and abstract dimensions of others' beliefs.
    Koster-Hale J; Richardson H; Velez N; Asaba M; Young L; Saxe R
    Neuroimage; 2017 Nov; 161():9-18. PubMed ID: 28807871
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 28. The neural correlates of passively viewed sequences of true and false beliefs.
    Geangu E; Gibson A; Kaduk K; Reid VM
    Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci; 2013 Apr; 8(4):432-7. PubMed ID: 22317745
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 29. No evidence that monkeys attribute mental states to animated shapes in the Heider-Simmel videos.
    Schafroth JL; Basile BM; Martin A; Murray EA
    Sci Rep; 2021 Feb; 11(1):3050. PubMed ID: 33542404
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 30. Flexible gaze-following in rhesus monkeys.
    Bettle R; Rosati AG
    Anim Cogn; 2019 Sep; 22(5):673-686. PubMed ID: 31098850
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 31. Macaques Exhibit Implicit Gaze Bias Anticipating Others' False-Belief-Driven Actions via Medial Prefrontal Cortex.
    Hayashi T; Akikawa R; Kawasaki K; Egawa J; Minamimoto T; Kobayashi K; Kato S; Hori Y; Nagai Y; Iijima A; Someya T; Hasegawa I
    Cell Rep; 2020 Mar; 30(13):4433-4444.e5. PubMed ID: 32234478
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 32. Eighteen-month-olds understand false beliefs in an unexpected-contents task.
    Buttelmann D; Over H; Carpenter M; Tomasello M
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2014 Mar; 119():120-6. PubMed ID: 24267393
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 33. Oxytocin improves behavioural and neural deficits in inferring others' social emotions in autism.
    Aoki Y; Yahata N; Watanabe T; Takano Y; Kawakubo Y; Kuwabara H; Iwashiro N; Natsubori T; Inoue H; Suga M; Takao H; Sasaki H; Gonoi W; Kunimatsu A; Kasai K; Yamasue H
    Brain; 2014 Nov; 137(Pt 11):3073-86. PubMed ID: 25149412
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 34. Comparison of human infants and rhesus monkeys on Piaget's AB task: evidence for dependence on dorsolateral prefrontal cortex.
    Diamond A; Goldman-Rakic PS
    Exp Brain Res; 1989; 74(1):24-40. PubMed ID: 2924839
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 35. The effect of a looker's past reliability on infants' reasoning about beliefs.
    Poulin-Dubois D; Chow V
    Dev Psychol; 2009 Nov; 45(6):1576-82. PubMed ID: 19899915
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 36. Animal cognition: monkeys recall previously seen images.
    Schwartz BL
    Curr Biol; 2011 May; 21(9):R311-3. PubMed ID: 21549950
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 37. The evolutionary origins of natural pedagogy: Rhesus monkeys show sustained attention following nonsocial cues versus social communicative signals.
    Bettle R; Rosati AG
    Dev Sci; 2021 Jan; 24(1):e12987. PubMed ID: 32412163
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 38. Is Language Required to Represent Others' Mental States? Evidence From Beliefs and Other Representations.
    Samuel S; Durdevic K; Legg EW; Lurz R; Clayton NS
    Cogn Sci; 2019 Jan; 43(1):. PubMed ID: 30648802
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 39. What Cognitive Representations Support Primate Theory of Mind?
    Martin A; Santos LR
    Trends Cogn Sci; 2016 May; 20(5):375-382. PubMed ID: 27052723
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 40. Seeing the World From Others' Perspective: 14-Month-Olds Show Altercentric Modulation Effects by Others' Beliefs.
    Kampis D; Kovács ÁM
    Open Mind (Camb); 2021; 5():189-207. PubMed ID: 36438424
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Previous]   [Next]    [New Search]
    of 11.