These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.
229 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 22253181)
1. Useful theories make predictions. Howes A Top Cogn Sci; 2012 Jan; 4(1):84-6; discussion 94-102. PubMed ID: 22253181 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
2. The complex systems approach: rhetoric or revolution. Eliasmith C Top Cogn Sci; 2012 Jan; 4(1):72-7; discussion 94-102. PubMed ID: 22253179 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
3. Self-organization takes time too. van Rooij I Top Cogn Sci; 2012 Jan; 4(1):63-71; discussion 94-102. PubMed ID: 22253178 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
4. Commentary: why I am not a dynamicist. Botvinick M Top Cogn Sci; 2012 Jan; 4(1):78-83; discussion 94-102. PubMed ID: 22253180 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
5. Abstract concepts require concrete models: why cognitive scientists have not yet embraced nonlinearly coupled, dynamical, self-organized critical, synergistic, scale-free, exquisitely context-sensitive, interaction-dominant, multifractal, interdependent brain-body-niche systems. Wagenmakers EJ; van der Maas HL; Farrell S Top Cogn Sci; 2012 Jan; 4(1):87-93; discussion 94-102. PubMed ID: 22253182 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
6. Multifractal dynamics in the emergence of cognitive structure. Dixon JA; Holden JG; Mirman D; Stephen DG Top Cogn Sci; 2012 Jan; 4(1):51-62. PubMed ID: 22253177 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
7. Complexity and extended phenomenological-cognitive systems. Silberstein M; Chemero A Top Cogn Sci; 2012 Jan; 4(1):35-50. PubMed ID: 22253176 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
8. Learning from the body about the mind. Riley MA; Shockley K; Van Orden G Top Cogn Sci; 2012 Jan; 4(1):21-34. PubMed ID: 22253175 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
9. Parallel Distributed Processing at 25: further explorations in the microstructure of cognition. Rogers TT; McClelland JL Cogn Sci; 2014 Aug; 38(6):1024-77. PubMed ID: 25087578 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
10. Human cognition and a pile of sand: a discussion on serial correlations and self-organized criticality. Wagenmakers EJ; Farrell S; Ratcliff R J Exp Psychol Gen; 2005 Feb; 134(1):108-16. PubMed ID: 15702966 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
11. Explaining how the mind works: on the relation between cognitive science and philosophy. Trigg J; Kalish M Top Cogn Sci; 2011 Apr; 3(2):399-424. PubMed ID: 25164301 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
12. A connectionist theory of phenomenal experience. O'Brien G; Opie J Behav Brain Sci; 1999 Feb; 22(1):127-48; discussion 148-96. PubMed ID: 11301521 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
13. Moving beyond computational cognitivism: understanding intentionality, intersubjectivity and ecology of mind. Ibáñez A; Cosmelli D Integr Psychol Behav Sci; 2008 Jun; 42(2):129-36. PubMed ID: 18196356 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
15. Categorial compositionality: a category theory explanation for the systematicity of human cognition. Phillips S; Wilson WH PLoS Comput Biol; 2010 Jul; 6(7):e1000858. PubMed ID: 20661306 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
16. Connecting cognition and consumer choice. Bartels DM; Johnson EJ Cognition; 2015 Feb; 135():47-51. PubMed ID: 25527275 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
17. Where does cognition occur: in one's head or in one's embodied/extended environment? Tibbetts PE Q Rev Biol; 2014 Dec; 89(4):359-68. PubMed ID: 25510079 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
18. From computing with numbers to computing with words. From manipulation of measurements to manipulation of perceptions. Zadeh LA Ann N Y Acad Sci; 2001 Apr; 929():221-52. PubMed ID: 11357866 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
20. Is cognitive science usefully cast as complexity science? Van Orden G; Stephen DG Top Cogn Sci; 2012 Jan; 4(1):3-6. PubMed ID: 22253173 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related] [Next] [New Search]