These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

157 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 2230660)

  • 1. Selective associations in the observational conditioning of fear in rhesus monkeys.
    Cook M; Mineka S
    J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process; 1990 Oct; 16(4):372-89. PubMed ID: 2230660
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 2. Observational conditioning of fear to fear-relevant versus fear-irrelevant stimuli in rhesus monkeys.
    Cook M; Mineka S
    J Abnorm Psychol; 1989 Nov; 98(4):448-59. PubMed ID: 2592680
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 3. Mechanisms involved in the observational conditioning of fear.
    Mineka S; Cook M
    J Exp Psychol Gen; 1993 Mar; 122(1):23-38. PubMed ID: 8440976
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 4. Emotional conditioning to masked stimuli and modulation of visuospatial attention.
    Beaver JD; Mogg K; Bradley BP
    Emotion; 2005 Mar; 5(1):67-79. PubMed ID: 15755220
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 5. Rapid detection of snakes by Japanese monkeys (Macaca fuscata): an evolutionarily predisposed visual system.
    Shibasaki M; Kawai N
    J Comp Psychol; 2009 May; 123(2):131-5. PubMed ID: 19450020
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 6. Snakes and cats in the flower bed: fast detection is not specific to pictures of fear-relevant animals.
    Lipp OV; Derakshan N; Waters AM; Logies S
    Emotion; 2004 Sep; 4(3):233-50. PubMed ID: 15456393
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 7. Blocked and overshadowed stimuli are weakened in their ability to serve as blockers and second-order reinforcers in Pavlovian fear conditioning.
    Rauhut AS; McPhee JE; Ayres JJ
    J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process; 1999 Jan; 25(1):45-67. PubMed ID: 9987858
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 8. Of snakes and succor: learning secure attachment associations with novel faces via negative stimulus pairings.
    Beckes L; Simpson JA; Erickson A
    Psychol Sci; 2010 May; 21(5):721-8. PubMed ID: 20483852
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 9. Of snakes and flowers: does preferential detection of pictures of fear-relevant animals in visual search reflect on fear-relevance?
    Lipp OV
    Emotion; 2006 May; 6(2):296-308. PubMed ID: 16768561
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 10. [Is there a feature-positive effect in classical conditioning in humans?].
    Deubner R; Lachnit H
    Z Exp Angew Psychol; 1994; 41(1):1-16. PubMed ID: 8178536
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 11. Associative processes in differentially reared rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta): blocking.
    Beauchamp AJ; Gluck JP; Fouty HE; Lewis MH
    Dev Psychobiol; 1991 Apr; 24(3):175-89. PubMed ID: 1936581
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 12. Effects of contextual conditioning and unconditional stimulus presentation on performance in appetitive conditioning.
    Bouton ME; Rosengard C; Achenbach GG; Peck CA; Brooks DC
    Q J Exp Psychol B; 1993 Feb; 46(1):63-95. PubMed ID: 8456178
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 13. Predicting fear of heights, snakes, and public speaking from multimodal classical conditioning events.
    Wu NY; Conger AJ; Dygdon JA
    Med Sci Monit; 2006 Apr; 12(4):CR159-67. PubMed ID: 16572050
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 14. Automatic attention does not equal automatic fear: preferential attention without implicit valence.
    Purkis HM; Lipp OV
    Emotion; 2007 May; 7(2):314-23. PubMed ID: 17516810
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 15. Stimulus generalization as a function of stimulus novelty and familiarity in rats.
    Honey RC
    J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process; 1990 Apr; 16(2):178-84. PubMed ID: 2335771
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 16. The narrow fellow in the grass: human infants associate snakes and fear.
    Deloache JS; Lobue V
    Dev Sci; 2009 Jan; 12(1):201-7. PubMed ID: 19120429
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 17. Preparedness for action: responding to the snake in the grass.
    Flykt A
    Am J Psychol; 2006; 119(1):29-43. PubMed ID: 16550854
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 18. Learning from others to cope with biting flies: social learning of fear-induced conditioned analgesia and active avoidance.
    Kavaliers M; Choleris E; Colwell DD
    Behav Neurosci; 2001 Jun; 115(3):661-74. PubMed ID: 11439455
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 19. Stimulus salience and negative patterning.
    Redhead ES; Pearce JM
    Q J Exp Psychol B; 1995 Feb; 48(1):67-83. PubMed ID: 7740125
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 20. Japanese monkeys (Macaca fuscata) quickly detect snakes but not spiders: Evolutionary origins of fear-relevant animals.
    Kawai N; Koda H
    J Comp Psychol; 2016 Aug; 130(3):299-303. PubMed ID: 27078076
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Next]    [New Search]
    of 8.