These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

308 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 22356178)

  • 21. Dogs' (Canis familiaris) responsiveness to human pointing gestures.
    Soproni K; Miklósi A; Topál J; Csányi V
    J Comp Psychol; 2002 Mar; 116(1):27-34. PubMed ID: 11926681
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 22. The response of guide dogs and pet dogs (Canis familiaris) to cues of human referential communication (pointing and gaze).
    Ittyerah M; Gaunet F
    Anim Cogn; 2009 Mar; 12(2):257-65. PubMed ID: 18769948
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 23. Adapting to the human world: dogs' responsiveness to our social cues.
    Reid PJ
    Behav Processes; 2009 Mar; 80(3):325-33. PubMed ID: 19056474
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 24. What or where? The meaning of referential human pointing for dogs (Canis familiaris).
    Tauzin T; Csík A; Kis A; Topál J
    J Comp Psychol; 2015 Nov; 129(4):334-8. PubMed ID: 26147704
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 25. Why do dogs (Canis familiaris) select the empty container in an observational learning task?
    Kupán K; Miklósi Á; Gergely G; Topál J
    Anim Cogn; 2011 Mar; 14(2):259-68. PubMed ID: 21140185
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 26. Selective responding to human ostensive communication is an early developing capacity of domestic dogs.
    Byosiere SE; Mundry R; Range F; Virányi Z
    Dev Sci; 2023 Jul; 26(4):e13361. PubMed ID: 36545915
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 27. Development of using experimenter-given cues in infant chimpanzees: longitudinal changes in behavior and cognitive development.
    Okamoto-Barth S; Tomonaga M; Tanaka M; Matsuzawa T
    Dev Sci; 2008 Jan; 11(1):98-108. PubMed ID: 18171372
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 28. Dogs (Canis familiaris), but not chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes), understand imperative pointing.
    Kirchhofer KC; Zimmermann F; Kaminski J; Tomasello M
    PLoS One; 2012; 7(2):e30913. PubMed ID: 22347411
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 29. Domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) use human gestures but not nonhuman tokens to find hidden food.
    Udell MA; Giglio RF; Wynne CD
    J Comp Psychol; 2008 Feb; 122(1):84-93. PubMed ID: 18298285
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 30. Does affective information influence domestic dogs' (Canis lupus familiaris) point-following behavior?
    Flom R; Gartman P
    Anim Cogn; 2016 Mar; 19(2):317-27. PubMed ID: 26515451
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 31. Obey or not obey? Dogs (Canis familiaris) behave differently in response to attentional states of their owners.
    Schwab C; Huber L
    J Comp Psychol; 2006 Aug; 120(3):169-75. PubMed ID: 16893253
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 32. Cross-species referential signalling events in domestic dogs (Canis familiaris).
    Worsley HK; O'Hara SJ
    Anim Cogn; 2018 Jul; 21(4):457-465. PubMed ID: 29713846
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 33. Dogs do not demonstrate a human-like bias to defer to communicative cues.
    Johnston AM; Huang Y; Santos LR
    Learn Behav; 2018 Dec; 46(4):449-461. PubMed ID: 30112598
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 34. Comprehension of human communicative signs in pet dogs (Canis familiaris).
    Soproni K; Miklósi A; Topál J; Csányi V
    J Comp Psychol; 2001 Jun; 115(2):122-6. PubMed ID: 11459158
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 35. Communication between domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) and humans: dogs are good learners.
    Elgier AM; Jakovcevic A; Barrera G; Mustaca AE; Bentosela M
    Behav Processes; 2009 Jul; 81(3):402-8. PubMed ID: 19520240
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 36. A comparison of pet and purpose-bred research dog (Canis familiaris) performance on human-guided object-choice tasks.
    Lazarowski L; Dorman DC
    Behav Processes; 2015 Jan; 110():60-7. PubMed ID: 25245303
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 37. Young children follow pointing over words in interpreting acts of reference.
    Grassmann S; Tomasello M
    Dev Sci; 2010 Jan; 13(1):252-63. PubMed ID: 20121881
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 38. Gestural communication in subadult bonobos (Pan paniscus): repertoire and use.
    Pika S; Liebal K; Tomasello M
    Am J Primatol; 2005 Jan; 65(1):39-61. PubMed ID: 15645456
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 39. Reassessing shelter dogs' use of human communicative cues in the standard object-choice task.
    Osborne T; Mulcahy NJ
    PLoS One; 2019; 14(3):e0213166. PubMed ID: 30845205
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 40. The emergence of social cognition in three young chimpanzees.
    Tomasello M; Carpenter M
    Monogr Soc Res Child Dev; 2005; 70(1):vii-132. PubMed ID: 16156847
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Previous]   [Next]    [New Search]
    of 16.