These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

161 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 22429031)

  • 101. Why are we good at detecting cheaters? A reply to Fodor.
    Beaman CP
    Cognition; 2002 Mar; 83(2):215-20; discussion 221. PubMed ID: 11869725
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 102. In defense of core competencies, quantitative change, and continuity.
    Quinn PC
    Child Dev; 2008; 79(6):1633-8. PubMed ID: 19037937
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 103. Humor, abstraction, and disbelief.
    Hoicka E; Jutsum S; Gattis M
    Cogn Sci; 2008 Sep; 32(6):985-1002. PubMed ID: 21585438
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 104. On the reality of the incorporeal intelligibles: a reflection on the metaphysics of psychology.
    Bakan D
    Percept Mot Skills; 2001 Oct; 93(2):531-40. PubMed ID: 11769910
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 105. TOWARD A MENTALISTIC ACCOUNT OF EARLY PSYCHOLOGICAL REASONING.
    Luo Y; Baillargeon R
    Curr Dir Psychol Sci; 2010 Oct; 19(5):301-307. PubMed ID: 21660130
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 106. Shape Measures for Triangles.
    Farin G
    IEEE Trans Vis Comput Graph; 2012 Jan; 18(1):43-6. PubMed ID: 21149882
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 107. Evaluative contexts facilitate implicit mentalizing: relation to the broader autism phenotype and mental health.
    Wu R; Leow K; Yu N; Rafter C; Rosenbaum K; F de C Hamilton A; White SJ
    Sci Rep; 2024 Feb; 14(1):4697. PubMed ID: 38409351
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 108. Coregistration of EEG and eye-tracking in infants and developing populations.
    Kulke L
    Atten Percept Psychophys; 2024 Feb; ():. PubMed ID: 38388851
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 109. False belief understanding in deaf children: what are the difficulties?
    Meristo M; Surian L; Strid K
    Front Psychol; 2024; 15():1238505. PubMed ID: 38304920
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 110. Do young children track other's beliefs, or merely their perceptual access? An interactive, anticipatory measure of early theory of mind.
    Barone P; Wenzel L; Proft M; Rakoczy H
    R Soc Open Sci; 2022 Oct; 9(10):211278. PubMed ID: 36226128
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 111. Development of cool and hot theory of mind and cool and hot inhibitory control abilities from 3.5 to 6.5 years of age.
    Draperi M; Aïte A; Cassotti M; Le Stanc L; Houdé O; Borst G
    PLoS One; 2022; 17(1):e0262251. PubMed ID: 35085269
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 112. Worse Theory of Mind in Only-Children Compared to Children With Siblings and Its Intervention.
    Zhang Z; Yu H; Long M; Li H
    Front Psychol; 2021; 12():754168. PubMed ID: 34899495
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 113. Perceptual Access Reasoning (PAR) in Developing a Representational Theory of Mind.
    Fabricius WV; Gonzales CR; Pesch A; Weimer AA; Pugliese J; Carroll K; Bolnick RR; Kupfer AS; Eisenberg N; Spinrad TL
    Monogr Soc Res Child Dev; 2021 Sep; 86(3):7-154. PubMed ID: 34580875
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 114. Implicit Theory of Mind under realistic social circumstances measured with mobile eye-tracking.
    Kulke L; Hinrichs MAB
    Sci Rep; 2021 Jan; 11(1):1215. PubMed ID: 33441890
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 115. Infants' performance in the indirect false belief tasks: A second-person interpretation.
    Barone P; Gomila A
    Wiley Interdiscip Rev Cogn Sci; 2021 May; 12(3):e1551. PubMed ID: 33319503
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 116. How Do Object Shape, Semantic Cues, and Apparent Velocity Affect the Attribution of Intentionality to Figures With Different Types of Movements?
    Morales-Bader D; Castillo RD; Olivares C; Miño F
    Front Psychol; 2020; 11():935. PubMed ID: 32477225
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 117. Fourteen- to Eighteen-Month-Old Infants Use Explicit Linguistic Information to Update an Agent's False Belief.
    Jin KS; Kim Y; Song M; Kim YJ; Lee H; Lee Y; Cha M; Song HJ
    Front Psychol; 2019; 10():2508. PubMed ID: 31824369
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 118. Is implicit Theory of Mind real but hard to detect? Testing adults with different stimulus materials.
    Kulke L; Wübker M; Rakoczy H
    R Soc Open Sci; 2019 Jul; 6(7):190068. PubMed ID: 31417713
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 119. Why can some implicit Theory of Mind tasks be replicated and others cannot? A test of mentalizing versus submentalizing accounts.
    Kulke L; Johannsen J; Rakoczy H
    PLoS One; 2019; 14(3):e0213772. PubMed ID: 30909288
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 120. Longitudinal evidence for 4-year-olds' but not 2- and 3-year-olds' false belief-related action anticipation.
    Grosse Wiesmann C; Friederici AD; Disla D; Steinbeis N; Singer T
    Cogn Dev; 2018; 46():58-68. PubMed ID: 30147231
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Previous]   [Next]    [New Search]
    of 9.