These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

113 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 22435844)

  • 1. Gaining control over responses to implicit attitude tests: Implementation intentions engender fast responses on attitude-incongruent trials.
    Webb TL; Sheeran P; Pepper J
    Br J Soc Psychol; 2012 Mar; 51(1):13-32. PubMed ID: 22435844
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 2. Figure-ground asymmetries in the Implicit Association Test (IAT).
    Rothermund K; Wentura D
    Z Exp Psychol; 2001; 48(2):94-106. PubMed ID: 11392986
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 3. Reducing the influence of extrapersonal associations on the Implicit Association Test: personalizing the IAT.
    Olson MA; Fazio RH
    J Pers Soc Psychol; 2004 May; 86(5):653-67. PubMed ID: 15161392
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 4. Intentional faking of the single category Implicit Association Test and the Implicit Association Test.
    Stieger S; Göritz AS; Hergovich A; Voracek M
    Psychol Rep; 2011 Aug; 109(1):219-30. PubMed ID: 22049663
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 5. Reducing the expression of implicit stereotypes: reflexive control through implementation intentions.
    Mendoza SA; Gollwitzer PM; Amodio DM
    Pers Soc Psychol Bull; 2010 Apr; 36(4):512-23. PubMed ID: 20363905
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 6. Positive implicit attitudes toward odor words.
    Bulsing PJ; Smeets MA; van den Hout MA
    Chem Senses; 2007 Jul; 32(6):525-34. PubMed ID: 17488744
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 7. Implementation Intentions Reduce Implicit Stereotype Activation and Application.
    Rees HR; Rivers AM; Sherman JW
    Pers Soc Psychol Bull; 2019 Jan; 45(1):37-53. PubMed ID: 29855220
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 8. When old and frail is not the same: dissociating category and stimulus effects in four implicit attitude measurement methods.
    Gast A; Rothermund K
    Q J Exp Psychol (Hove); 2010 Mar; 63(3):479-98. PubMed ID: 19606404
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 9. Discriminating between the effects of valence and salience in the Implicit Association Test.
    Chang BP; Mitchell CJ
    Q J Exp Psychol (Hove); 2011 Nov; 64(11):2251-75. PubMed ID: 21722058
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 10. Estimating the contributions of associations and recoding in the Implicit Association Test: the ReAL model for the IAT.
    Meissner F; Rothermund K
    J Pers Soc Psychol; 2013 Jan; 104(1):45-69. PubMed ID: 23148698
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 11. Items' cross-category associations as a confounding factor in the Implicit Association Test.
    Steffens MC; Plewe I
    Z Exp Psychol; 2001; 48(2):123-34. PubMed ID: 11392980
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 12. Conflict monitoring and stimulus categorization processes involved in the prosocial attitude implicit association test: Evidence from event-related potentials.
    Xiao F; Zheng Z; Wang Y; Cui J; Chen Y
    Soc Neurosci; 2015 Aug; 10(4):408-17. PubMed ID: 25623088
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 13. Implicit and explicit attitudes toward gay males and lesbians among heterosexual males and females.
    Breen AB; Karpinski A
    J Soc Psychol; 2013; 153(3):351-74. PubMed ID: 23724704
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 14. Measuring individual differences in implicit cognition: the implicit association test.
    Greenwald AG; McGhee DE; Schwartz JL
    J Pers Soc Psychol; 1998 Jun; 74(6):1464-80. PubMed ID: 9654756
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 15. What goes well with physics? Measuring and altering the image of science.
    Kessels U; Rau M; Hannover B
    Br J Educ Psychol; 2006 Dec; 76(Pt 4):761-80. PubMed ID: 17094885
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 16. Base rate effects on the IAT.
    Bluemke M; Fiedler K
    Conscious Cogn; 2009 Dec; 18(4):1029-38. PubMed ID: 19700351
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 17. Minimizing the influence of recoding in the Implicit Association Test: the Recoding-Free Implicit Association Test (IAT-RF).
    Rothermund K; Teige-Mocigemba S; Gast A; Wentura D
    Q J Exp Psychol (Hove); 2009 Jan; 62(1):84-98. PubMed ID: 18609400
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 18. Application of implicit attitude measures to the blood donation context.
    Warfel RM; France CR; France JL
    Transfusion; 2012 Feb; 52(2):356-65. PubMed ID: 21827501
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 19. Attitudinal and non-attitudinal components of IAT performance.
    Calanchini J; Sherman JW; Klauer KC; Lai CK
    Pers Soc Psychol Bull; 2014 Oct; 40(10):1285-96. PubMed ID: 24986841
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 20. Implicit Association Test as an Analogical Learning Task.
    Hussey I; De Houwer J
    Exp Psychol; 2018 Sep; 65(5):272-285. PubMed ID: 30451105
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Next]    [New Search]
    of 6.