BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

243 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 22533318)

  • 21. Modulation of the cortical false belief network during development.
    Sommer M; Meinhardt J; Eichenmüller K; Sodian B; Döhnel K; Hajak G
    Brain Res; 2010 Oct; 1354():123-31. PubMed ID: 20678489
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 22. Individual differences in executive functioning predict preschoolers' improvement from theory-of-mind training.
    Benson JE; Sabbagh MA; Carlson SM; Zelazo PD
    Dev Psychol; 2013 Sep; 49(9):1615-27. PubMed ID: 23244411
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 23. A child-centred exploration of the relevance of family and friends to theory of mind development.
    Wright BC; Mahfoud J
    Scand J Psychol; 2012 Feb; 53(1):32-40. PubMed ID: 22022976
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 24. Metacognitive development of deaf children: lessons from the appearance-reality and false belief tasks.
    Courtin C; Melot AM
    Dev Sci; 2005 Jan; 8(1):16-25. PubMed ID: 15647063
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 25. Children exhibit different performance patterns in explicit and implicit theory of mind tasks.
    Oktay-Gür N; Schulz A; Rakoczy H
    Cognition; 2018 Apr; 173():60-74. PubMed ID: 29331664
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 26. Theory of Mind in Parkinson's disease.
    Freedman M; Stuss DT
    J Neurol Sci; 2011 Nov; 310(1-2):225-7. PubMed ID: 21705020
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 27. The relation between individual differences in fantasy and theory of mind.
    Taylor M; Carlson SM
    Child Dev; 1997 Jun; 68(3):436-55. PubMed ID: 9249959
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 28. Declarative joint attention as a foundation of theory of mind.
    Sodian B; Kristen-Antonow S
    Dev Psychol; 2015 Sep; 51(9):1190-200. PubMed ID: 26192041
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 29. An association account of false belief understanding.
    De Bruin LC; Newen A
    Cognition; 2012 May; 123(2):240-59. PubMed ID: 22297384
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 30. Ways of thinking: from crows to children and back again.
    Clayton NS
    Q J Exp Psychol (Hove); 2015; 68(2):209-41. PubMed ID: 25207993
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 31. Children's level of participation in a false-belief task, age, and theory of mind.
    Ritblatt SN
    J Genet Psychol; 2000 Mar; 161(1):53-64. PubMed ID: 10705584
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 32. The relation between acquisition of a theory of mind and the capacity to hold in mind.
    Gordon AC; Olson DR
    J Exp Child Psychol; 1998 Jan; 68(1):70-83. PubMed ID: 9473316
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 33. Online usage of theory of mind continues to develop in late adolescence.
    Dumontheil I; Apperly IA; Blakemore SJ
    Dev Sci; 2010 Mar; 13(2):331-8. PubMed ID: 20136929
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 34. Beyond Simulation-Theory and Theory-Theory: why social cognitive neuroscience should use its own concepts to study "theory of mind".
    Apperly IA
    Cognition; 2008 Apr; 107(1):266-83. PubMed ID: 17868666
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 35. Intergenerational transmission of theory-of-mind.
    Sabbagh MA; Seamans EL
    Dev Sci; 2008 May; 11(3):354-60. PubMed ID: 18466369
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 36. Rethinking the cognitive revolution from a neural perspective: how overuse/misuse of the term 'cognition' and the neglect of affective controls in behavioral neuroscience could be delaying progress in understanding the BrainMind.
    Cromwell HC; Panksepp J
    Neurosci Biobehav Rev; 2011 Oct; 35(9):2026-35. PubMed ID: 21345347
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 37. Implicit and explicit theory of mind: state of the art.
    Low J; Perner J
    Br J Dev Psychol; 2012 Mar; 30(Pt 1):1-13. PubMed ID: 22429029
    [No Abstract]   [Full Text] [Related]  

  • 38. Where will the triangle look for it? Attributing false beliefs to a geometric shape at 17 months.
    Surian L; Geraci A
    Br J Dev Psychol; 2012 Mar; 30(Pt 1):30-44. PubMed ID: 22429031
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 39. Impaired social behavior in children with benign childhood epilepsy with centrotemporal spikes.
    Genizi J; Shamay-Tsoory SG; Shahar E; Yaniv S; Aharon-Perez J
    J Child Neurol; 2012 Feb; 27(2):156-61. PubMed ID: 21868370
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 40. Social cognition is not reducible to theory of mind: when children use deontic rules to predict the behaviour of others.
    Clément F; Bernard S; Kaufmann L
    Br J Dev Psychol; 2011 Nov; 29(Pt 4):910-28. PubMed ID: 21995744
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Previous]   [Next]    [New Search]
    of 13.