These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

145 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 22582816)

  • 1. Choice behavior of pigeons (Columba livia), college students, and preschool children (Homo sapiens) in the Monty Hall dilemma.
    Mazur JE; Kahlbaugh PE
    J Comp Psychol; 2012 Nov; 126(4):407-20. PubMed ID: 22582816
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 2. The Monty Hall dilemma with pigeons: No, you choose for me.
    Zentall TR; Case JP; Collins TL
    Learn Behav; 2015 Sep; 43(3):209-16. PubMed ID: 25772336
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 3. Are birds smarter than mathematicians? Pigeons (Columba livia) perform optimally on a version of the Monty Hall Dilemma.
    Herbranson WT; Schroeder J
    J Comp Psychol; 2010 Feb; 124(1):1-13. PubMed ID: 20175592
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 4. Further investigation of the Monty Hall Dilemma in pigeons and rats.
    Stagner JP; Zentall TR
    Behav Processes; 2015 Mar; 112():14-21. PubMed ID: 25452075
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 5. Testing the limits of optimality: the effect of base rates in the Monty Hall dilemma.
    Herbranson WT; Wang S
    Learn Behav; 2014 Mar; 42(1):69-82. PubMed ID: 24234041
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 6. The Monty Hall dilemma in pigeons: effect of investment in initial choice.
    Stagner JP; Rayburn-Reeves R; Zentall TR
    Psychon Bull Rev; 2013 Oct; 20(5):997-1004. PubMed ID: 23430792
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 7. Learning how to "make a deal": human (Homo sapiens) and monkey (Macaca mulatta) performance when repeatedly faced with the Monty Hall Dilemma.
    Klein ED; Evans TA; Schultz NB; Beran MJ
    J Comp Psychol; 2013 Feb; 127(1):103-8. PubMed ID: 22746157
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 8. Brain activities associated with learning of the Monty Hall Dilemma task.
    Hirao T; Murphy TI; Masaki H
    Psychophysiology; 2017 Sep; 54(9):1359-1369. PubMed ID: 28480973
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 9. End the search quickly: Pigeons (Columba livia) and humans (Homo sapiens) share the same bias.
    Fragaszy DM
    J Comp Psychol; 2022 Feb; 136(1):1-2. PubMed ID: 35025549
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 10. Like chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes), pigeons (Columba livia domestica) match and Nash equilibrate where humans (Homo sapiens) do not.
    Hachiga Y; Schwartz LP; Tripoli C; Michaels S; Kearns D; Silberberg A
    J Comp Psychol; 2019 May; 133(2):197-206. PubMed ID: 30372107
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 11. Should I stay or should I go? Pigeons' (Columba livia) performance of a foraging task has implications for optimal foraging theory and serial pattern learning.
    Chandel H; Boring M; Zentall TR; Wasserman EA
    J Comp Psychol; 2021 May; 135(2):266-272. PubMed ID: 33523682
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 12. Sunk cost: pigeons (Columba livia), too, show bias to complete a task rather than shift to another.
    Pattison KF; Zentall TR; Watanabe S
    J Comp Psychol; 2012 Feb; 126(1):1-9. PubMed ID: 21574688
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 13. The power of nothing: Risk preference in pigeons, but not people, is driven primarily by avoidance of zero outcomes.
    Pisklak JM; Madan CR; Ludvig EA; Spetch ML
    J Exp Psychol Anim Learn Cogn; 2019 Oct; 45(4):431-445. PubMed ID: 31414880
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 14. Early commitment facilitates optimal choice by pigeons.
    Zentall TR; Case JP; Berry JR
    Psychon Bull Rev; 2017 Jun; 24(3):957-963. PubMed ID: 27743217
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 15. Perception of the standard and the reversed Müller-Lyer figures in pigeons (Columba livia) and humans (Homo sapiens).
    Nakamura N; Fujita K; Ushitani T; Miyata H
    J Comp Psychol; 2006 Aug; 120(3):252-61. PubMed ID: 16893262
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 16. Suboptimal choice behavior by pigeons.
    Stagner JP; Zentall TR
    Psychon Bull Rev; 2010 Jun; 17(3):412-6. PubMed ID: 20551367
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 17. Pigeon's (Columba livia) paradoxical preference for the suboptimal alternative in a complex foraging task.
    Zentall TR; Case JP; Luong J
    J Comp Psychol; 2016 May; 130(2):138-44. PubMed ID: 27064201
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 18. The paradoxical performance by different species on the ephemeral reward task.
    Zentall TR
    Learn Behav; 2021 Mar; 49(1):99-105. PubMed ID: 32583140
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 19. Frequency and value both matter in the suboptimal choice procedure.
    Pisklak JM; McDevitt MA; Dunn RM; Spetch ML
    J Exp Anal Behav; 2019 Jan; 111(1):1-11. PubMed ID: 30569554
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 20. Enhancing "self-control": The paradoxical effect of delay of reinforcement.
    Zentall TR
    Learn Behav; 2020 Mar; 48(1):165-172. PubMed ID: 32026400
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Next]    [New Search]
    of 8.