These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

134 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 22642888)

  • 1. Direct measurement of spontaneous strategy selection in a virtual Morris water maze shows females choose an allocentric strategy at least as often as males do.
    van Gerven DJ; Schneider AN; Wuitchik DM; Skelton RW
    Behav Neurosci; 2012 Jun; 126(3):465-78. PubMed ID: 22642888
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 2. Human navigation that requires calculating heading vectors recruits parietal cortex in a virtual and visually sparse water maze task in fMRI.
    Rodriguez PF
    Behav Neurosci; 2010 Aug; 124(4):532-40. PubMed ID: 20695652
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 3. Sex differences in a virtual water maze: an eye tracking and pupillometry study.
    Mueller SC; Jackson CP; Skelton RW
    Behav Brain Res; 2008 Nov; 193(2):209-15. PubMed ID: 18602173
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 4. Sex differences in human virtual water maze performance: novel measures reveal the relative contribution of directional responding and spatial knowledge.
    Woolley DG; Vermaercke B; Op de Beeck H; Wagemans J; Gantois I; D'Hooge R; Swinnen SP; Wenderoth N
    Behav Brain Res; 2010 Apr; 208(2):408-14. PubMed ID: 20035800
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 5. Simple gaze analysis and special design of a virtual Morris water maze provides a new method for differentiating egocentric and allocentric navigational strategy choice.
    Livingstone-Lee SA; Murchison S; Zeman PM; Gandhi M; van Gerven D; Stewart L; Livingston NJ; Skelton RW
    Behav Brain Res; 2011 Nov; 225(1):117-25. PubMed ID: 21771614
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 6. Sex differences and correlations in a virtual Morris water task, a virtual radial arm maze, and mental rotation.
    Astur RS; Tropp J; Sava S; Constable RT; Markus EJ
    Behav Brain Res; 2004 May; 151(1-2):103-15. PubMed ID: 15084426
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 7. Acute stress switches spatial navigation strategy from egocentric to allocentric in a virtual Morris water maze.
    van Gerven DJH; Ferguson T; Skelton RW
    Neurobiol Learn Mem; 2016 Jul; 132():29-39. PubMed ID: 27174311
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 8. Incidental learning of allocentric and egocentric strategies by both men and women in a dual-strategy virtual Morris Water Maze.
    Ferguson TD; Livingstone-Lee SA; Skelton RW
    Behav Brain Res; 2019 May; 364():281-295. PubMed ID: 30794853
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 9. Spontaneous navigational strategies and performance in the virtual town.
    Etchamendy N; Bohbot VD
    Hippocampus; 2007; 17(8):595-9. PubMed ID: 17546682
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 10. Developmental time course of the acquisition of sequential egocentric and allocentric navigation strategies.
    Bullens J; Iglói K; Berthoz A; Postma A; Rondi-Reig L
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2010 Nov; 107(3):337-50. PubMed ID: 20598705
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 11. The effects of overtraining in the Morris water maze on allocentric and egocentric learning strategies in rats.
    Kealy J; Diviney M; Kehoe E; McGonagle V; O'Shea A; Harvey D; Commins S
    Behav Brain Res; 2008 Oct; 192(2):259-63. PubMed ID: 18514924
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 12. Navigating in a virtual three-dimensional maze: how do egocentric and allocentric reference frames interact?
    Vidal M; Amorim MA; Berthoz A
    Brain Res Cogn Brain Res; 2004 May; 19(3):244-58. PubMed ID: 15062862
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 13. Sequential egocentric strategy is acquired as early as allocentric strategy: Parallel acquisition of these two navigation strategies.
    Iglói K; Zaoui M; Berthoz A; Rondi-Reig L
    Hippocampus; 2009 Dec; 19(12):1199-211. PubMed ID: 19360853
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 14. Sex differences in directional cue use in a virtual landscape.
    Chai XJ; Jacobs LF
    Behav Neurosci; 2009 Apr; 123(2):276-83. PubMed ID: 19331451
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 15. Emergence of an egocentric cue guiding and allocentric inferring strategy that mirrors hippocampal brain-derived neurotrophic factor (BDNF) expression in the Morris water maze.
    Harvey DR; McGauran AM; Murphy J; Burns L; McMonagle E; Commins S
    Neurobiol Learn Mem; 2008 May; 89(4):462-79. PubMed ID: 17923424
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 16. Spatial deficits in a virtual water maze in amnesic participants with hippocampal damage.
    Goodrich-Hunsaker NJ; Livingstone SA; Skelton RW; Hopkins RO
    Hippocampus; 2010 Apr; 20(4):481-91. PubMed ID: 19554566
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 17. Egocentric memory impaired and allocentric memory intact as assessed by virtual reality in subjects with unilateral parietal cortex lesions.
    Weniger G; Ruhleder M; Wolf S; Lange C; Irle E
    Neuropsychologia; 2009 Jan; 47(1):59-69. PubMed ID: 18789955
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 18. Hex Maze: A new virtual maze able to track acquisition and usage of three navigation strategies.
    Spriggs MJ; Kirk IJ; Skelton RW
    Behav Brain Res; 2018 Feb; 339():195-206. PubMed ID: 29203335
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 19. Eye tracking, strategies, and sex differences in virtual navigation.
    Andersen NE; Dahmani L; Konishi K; Bohbot VD
    Neurobiol Learn Mem; 2012 Jan; 97(1):81-9. PubMed ID: 22001012
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 20. Egocentric and allocentric navigation strategies in Williams syndrome and typical development.
    Broadbent HJ; Farran EK; Tolmie A
    Dev Sci; 2014 Nov; 17(6):920-34. PubMed ID: 24702907
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Next]    [New Search]
    of 7.