These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

220 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 22801861)

  • 1. Sequential responding and planning in capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella).
    Beran MJ; Parrish AE
    Anim Cogn; 2012 Nov; 15(6):1085-94. PubMed ID: 22801861
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 2. Metamemory in tufted capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella).
    Fujita K
    Anim Cogn; 2009 Jul; 12(4):575-85. PubMed ID: 19242741
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 3. Looking ahead? Computerized maze task performance by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes), rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta), capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella), and human children (Homo sapiens).
    Beran MJ; Parrish AE; Futch SE; Evans TA; Perdue BM
    J Comp Psychol; 2015 May; 129(2):160-73. PubMed ID: 25798793
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 4. Monkeys (Macaca mulatta and Cebus apella) track, enumerate, and compare multiple sets of moving items.
    Beran MJ
    J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process; 2008 Jan; 34(1):63-74. PubMed ID: 18248115
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 5. Identity concept learning in matching-to-sample tasks by tufted capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella).
    Truppa V; Garofoli D; Castorina G; Piano Mortari E; Natale F; Visalberghi E
    Anim Cogn; 2010 Nov; 13(6):835-48. PubMed ID: 20574839
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 6. Personality structure in brown capuchin monkeys (Sapajus apella): comparisons with chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes), orangutans (Pongo spp.), and rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta).
    Morton FB; Lee PC; Buchanan-Smith HM; Brosnan SF; Thierry B; Paukner A; de Waal FB; Widness J; Essler JL; Weiss A
    J Comp Psychol; 2013 Aug; 127(3):282-98. PubMed ID: 23668695
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 7. Capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella) succeed in a test of quantity conservation.
    Beran MJ
    Anim Cogn; 2008 Jan; 11(1):109-16. PubMed ID: 17549530
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 8. Delaying gratification for food and tokens in capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella) and chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes): when quantity is salient, symbolic stimuli do not improve performance.
    Evans TA; Beran MJ; Paglieri F; Addessi E
    Anim Cogn; 2012 Jul; 15(4):539-48. PubMed ID: 22434403
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 9. Capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella) modulate their use of an uncertainty response depending on risk.
    Beran MJ; Perdue BM; Church BA; Smith JD
    J Exp Psychol Anim Learn Cogn; 2016 Jan; 42(1):32-43. PubMed ID: 26551351
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 10. Ordinal judgments of symbolic stimuli by capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella) and rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta): the effects of differential and nondifferential reward.
    Beran MJ; Harris EH; Evans TA; Klein ED; Chan B; Flemming TM; Washburn DA
    J Comp Psychol; 2008 Feb; 122(1):52-61. PubMed ID: 18298281
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 11. What are my chances? Closing the gap in uncertainty monitoring between rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta) and capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella).
    Beran MJ; Perdue BM; Smith JD
    J Exp Psychol Anim Learn Cogn; 2014 Jul; 40(3):303-16. PubMed ID: 25368870
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 12. Ontogenetic dissociation between habit learning and recognition memory in capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella).
    Resende MC; Tavares MC; Tomaz C
    Neurobiol Learn Mem; 2003 Jan; 79(1):19-24. PubMed ID: 12482675
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 13. An assessment of memory awareness in tufted capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella).
    Basile BM; Hampton RR; Suomi SJ; Murray EA
    Anim Cogn; 2009 Jan; 12(1):169-80. PubMed ID: 18712532
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 14. Picture processing in tufted capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella).
    Truppa V; Spinozzi G; Stegagno T; Fagot J
    Behav Processes; 2009 Oct; 82(2):140-52. PubMed ID: 19501136
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 15. Sequential responding and planning in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta).
    Beran MJ; Pate JL; Washburn DA; Rumbaugh DM
    J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process; 2004 Jul; 30(3):203-12. PubMed ID: 15279511
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 16. Can old-world and new-world monkeys judge spatial above/below relations to be the same or different? Some of them, but not all of them.
    Thompson RK; Flemming TM; Hagmann CE
    Behav Processes; 2016 Feb; 123():74-83. PubMed ID: 26581319
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 17. Learning from others' mistakes in capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella).
    Kuroshima H; Kuwahata H; Fujita K
    Anim Cogn; 2008 Oct; 11(4):599-609. PubMed ID: 18320243
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 18. Do primates see the solitaire illusion differently? A comparative assessment of humans (Homo sapiens), chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes), rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta), and capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella).
    Agrillo C; Parrish AE; Beran MJ
    J Comp Psychol; 2014 Nov; 128(4):402-13. PubMed ID: 25133464
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 19. Conceptual thresholds for same and different in old-(Macaca mulatta) and new-world (Cebus apella) monkeys.
    Flemming TM
    Behav Processes; 2011 Mar; 86(3):316-22. PubMed ID: 21238555
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 20. Implicit and explicit category learning by capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella).
    Smith JD; Crossley MJ; Boomer J; Church BA; Beran MJ; Ashby FG
    J Comp Psychol; 2012 Aug; 126(3):294-304. PubMed ID: 22023264
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Next]    [New Search]
    of 11.