These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

110 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 22921368)

  • 1. Decision making during the psychological refractory period.
    Zylberberg A; Ouellette B; Sigman M; Roelfsema PR
    Curr Biol; 2012 Oct; 22(19):1795-9. PubMed ID: 22921368
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 2. Separate and shared sources of dual-task cost in stimulus identification and response selection.
    Arnell KM; Duncan J
    Cogn Psychol; 2002 Mar; 44(2):105-47. PubMed ID: 11863322
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 3. A shared cortical bottleneck underlying Attentional Blink and Psychological Refractory Period.
    Marti S; Sigman M; Dehaene S
    Neuroimage; 2012 Feb; 59(3):2883-98. PubMed ID: 21988891
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 4. When underadditivity of factor effects in the Psychological Refractory Period paradigm implies a bottleneck: evidence from psycholinguistics.
    Besner D; Reynolds M; O'Malley S
    Q J Exp Psychol (Hove); 2009 Nov; 62(11):2222-34. PubMed ID: 19370482
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 5. Queuing network modeling of the psychological refractory period (PRP).
    Wu C; Liu Y
    Psychol Rev; 2008 Oct; 115(4):913-54. PubMed ID: 18954209
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 6. Central interference in driving: is there any stopping the psychological refractory period?
    Levy J; Pashler H; Boer E
    Psychol Sci; 2006 Mar; 17(3):228-35. PubMed ID: 16507063
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 7. On the optimality of serial and parallel processing in the psychological refractory period paradigm: effects of the distribution of stimulus onset asynchronies.
    Miller J; Ulrich R; Rolke B
    Cogn Psychol; 2009 May; 58(3):273-310. PubMed ID: 19281972
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 8. Probing the cortical network underlying the psychological refractory period: a combined EEG-fMRI study.
    Hesselmann G; Flandin G; Dehaene S
    Neuroimage; 2011 Jun; 56(3):1608-21. PubMed ID: 21397701
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 9. Can practice eliminate the psychological refractory period effect?
    Van Selst M; Ruthruff E; Johnston JC
    J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform; 1999 Oct; 25(5):1268-83. PubMed ID: 10531663
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 10. Dual route for subtask order control: Evidence from the psychological refractory paradigm.
    Luria R; Meiran N
    Q J Exp Psychol (Hove); 2006 Apr; 59(4):720-44. PubMed ID: 16707359
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 11. The source of execution-related dual-task interference: motor bottleneck or response monitoring?
    Bratzke D; Rolke B; Ulrich R
    J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform; 2009 Oct; 35(5):1413-26. PubMed ID: 19803646
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 12. Procedural learning eliminates specific slowing down of response selection in patients with idiopathic Parkinson syndrome.
    Lachmann T; Schumacher B; Joebges M; Hummelsheim H; van Leeuwen C
    J Clin Exp Neuropsychol; 2008 Apr; 30(3):319-26. PubMed ID: 17852616
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 13. A psychological refractory period in access to visual short-term memory and the deployment of visual-spatial attention: multitasking processing deficits revealed by event-related potentials.
    Brisson B; Jolicoeur P
    Psychophysiology; 2007 Mar; 44(2):323-33. PubMed ID: 17343714
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 14. Noise and correlations in parallel perceptual decision making.
    Otto TU; Mamassian P
    Curr Biol; 2012 Aug; 22(15):1391-6. PubMed ID: 22771043
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 15. Response grouping in the psychological refractory period (PRP) paradigm: models and contamination effects.
    Ulrich R; Miller J
    Cogn Psychol; 2008 Sep; 57(2):75-121. PubMed ID: 18262510
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 16. Action-effect codes in and before the central bottleneck: evidence from the psychological refractory period paradigm.
    Paelecke M; Kunde W
    J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform; 2007 Jun; 33(3):627-44. PubMed ID: 17563226
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 17. Are spatial responses to visuospatial stimuli and spoken responses to auditory letters ideomotor-compatible tasks? Examination of set-size effects on dual-task interference.
    Shin YK; Proctor RW
    Acta Psychol (Amst); 2008 Nov; 129(3):352-64. PubMed ID: 18845280
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 18. Asymmetric interference in concurrent time-to-contact estimation: Cousin or twin of the psychological refractory period effect?
    Baurès R; DeLucia PR; Olson M; Oberfeld D
    Atten Percept Psychophys; 2017 Feb; 79(2):698-711. PubMed ID: 27896708
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 19. Mapping introspection's blind spot: reconstruction of dual-task phenomenology using quantified introspection.
    Marti S; Sackur J; Sigman M; Dehaene S
    Cognition; 2010 May; 115(2):303-13. PubMed ID: 20129603
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 20. Causal role of dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in human perceptual decision making.
    Philiastides MG; Auksztulewicz R; Heekeren HR; Blankenburg F
    Curr Biol; 2011 Jun; 21(11):980-3. PubMed ID: 21620706
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Next]    [New Search]
    of 6.