These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

150 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 22962623)

  • 1. Where do the eyes really go in the hollow-face illusion?
    Grosjean M; Rinkenauer G; Jainta S
    PLoS One; 2012; 7(9):e44706. PubMed ID: 22962623
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 2. Is prior knowledge of object geometry used in visually guided reaching?
    Hartung B; Schrater PR; Bülthoff HH; Kersten D; Franz VH
    J Vis; 2005 Jun; 5(6):504-14. PubMed ID: 16097863
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 3. Binocular disparity magnitude affects perceived depth magnitude despite inversion of depth order.
    Matthews H; Hill H; Palmisano S
    Perception; 2011; 40(8):975-88. PubMed ID: 22132512
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 4. Depth cues, rather than perceived depth, govern vergence.
    Wismeijer DA; van Ee R; Erkelens CJ
    Exp Brain Res; 2008 Jan; 184(1):61-70. PubMed ID: 17717656
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 5. Eye vergence is susceptible to the hollow-face illusion.
    Hoffmann J; Sebald A
    Perception; 2007; 36(3):461-70. PubMed ID: 17455759
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 6. Six-month-old infants perceive the hollow-face illusion.
    Corrow S; Granrud CE; Mathison J; Yonas A
    Perception; 2011; 40(11):1376-83. PubMed ID: 22416594
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 7. Experiments with a hollow mask and a reverspective: top-down influences in the inversion effect for 3-D stimuli.
    Papathomas TV; Bono LM
    Perception; 2004; 33(9):1129-38. PubMed ID: 15560511
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 8. Illusory depth perception of oblique lines produced by overlaid vertical disparity.
    Ito H
    Vision Res; 2005 Mar; 45(7):931-42. PubMed ID: 15644231
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 9. Shrinkage in the apparent size of cylindrical objects.
    Taya S; Miura K
    Perception; 2007; 36(1):3-16. PubMed ID: 17357702
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 10. [Pro-psychotic change of binocular depth inversion by sleep deprivation].
    Sternemann U; Schneider U; Leweke FM; Bevilacqua CM; Dietrich DE; Emrich HM
    Nervenarzt; 1997 Jul; 68(7):593-6. PubMed ID: 9333723
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 11. Monocular occlusion cues alter the influence of terminator motion in the barber pole phenomenon.
    Lidén L; Mingolla E
    Vision Res; 1998 Dec; 38(24):3883-98. PubMed ID: 10211381
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 12. Bayesian combination of ambiguous shape cues.
    Adams WJ; Mamassian P
    J Vis; 2004 Nov; 4(10):921-9. PubMed ID: 15595895
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 13. Impaired perceptual processing and conceptual cognition in patients with anxiety disorders: a pilot study with the binocular depth inversion paradigm.
    Passie T; Schneider U; Borsutzky M; Breyer R; Emrich HM; Bandelow B; Schmid-Ott G
    Psychol Health Med; 2013; 18(3):363-74. PubMed ID: 23186162
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 14. Six-month-old infants' perception of the hollow face illusion: evidence for a general convexity bias.
    Corrow SL; Mathison J; Granrud CE; Yonas A
    Perception; 2014; 43(11):1177-90. PubMed ID: 25638934
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 15. Interaction of stereo vision and optic flow processing revealed by an illusory stimulus.
    Grigo A; Lappe M
    Vision Res; 1998 Jan; 38(2):281-90. PubMed ID: 9536354
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 16. Focusing on an illusion: Accommodating to perceived depth?
    Koessler T; Hill H
    Vision Res; 2019 Jan; 154():131-141. PubMed ID: 30468753
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 17. Monocular occlusions determine the perceived shape and depth of occluding surfaces.
    Tsirlin I; Wilcox LM; Allison RS
    J Vis; 2010 Jun; 10(6):11. PubMed ID: 20884560
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 18. Unconscious adaptation: a new illusion of depth induced by stimulus features without depth.
    Hayashi R; Miyawaki Y; Maeda T; Tachi S
    Vision Res; 2003 Dec; 43(26):2773-82. PubMed ID: 14568094
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 19. Specular reflections and the estimation of shape from binocular disparity.
    Muryy AA; Welchman AE; Blake A; Fleming RW
    Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A; 2013 Feb; 110(6):2413-8. PubMed ID: 23341602
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 20. Using the kinetic Zollner illusion to quantify the interaction between form and motion information in depth.
    Khuu SK; Kim DD
    Vision Res; 2013 May; 83():48-55. PubMed ID: 23474298
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Next]    [New Search]
    of 8.