These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

182 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 23029341)

  • 1. What eye movements can tell about theory of mind in a strategic game.
    Meijering B; van Rijn H; Taatgen NA; Verbrugge R
    PLoS One; 2012; 7(9):e45961. PubMed ID: 23029341
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 2. Autism does not limit strategic thinking in the "beauty contest" game.
    Pantelis PC; Kennedy DP
    Cognition; 2017 Mar; 160():91-97. PubMed ID: 28081516
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 3. Perspective-taking and depth of theory-of-mind reasoning in sequential-move games.
    Zhang J; Hedden T; Chia A
    Cogn Sci; 2012 Apr; 36(3):560-73. PubMed ID: 22436023
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 4. Children's strategic theory of mind.
    Sher I; Koenig M; Rustichini A
    Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A; 2014 Sep; 111(37):13307-12. PubMed ID: 25197065
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 5. What do you think I think you think?: Strategic reasoning in matrix games.
    Hedden T; Zhang J
    Cognition; 2002 Aug; 85(1):1-36. PubMed ID: 12086711
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 6. Flexibility of theory of mind in a matrix game when the opponent's level changes.
    Li J; Liu XP; Tang WH
    Am J Psychol; 2014; 127(1):75-85. PubMed ID: 24720098
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 7. Experience and abstract reasoning in learning backward induction.
    Hawes DR; Vostroknutov A; Rustichini A
    Front Neurosci; 2012; 6():23. PubMed ID: 22363254
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 8. Flexibility of theory of mind in a matrix game when partner's level is different.
    Li J; Liu XP; Zhu L
    Psychol Rep; 2011 Oct; 109(2):675-85. PubMed ID: 22238865
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 9. Self-reported strategies in decisions under risk: role of feedback, reasoning abilities, executive functions, short-term-memory, and working memory.
    Schiebener J; Brand M
    Cogn Process; 2015 Nov; 16(4):401-16. PubMed ID: 26289475
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 10. A mediation model to explain decision making under conditions of risk among adolescents: the role of fluid intelligence and probabilistic reasoning.
    Donati MA; Panno A; Chiesi F; Primi C
    J Clin Exp Neuropsychol; 2014; 36(6):588-95. PubMed ID: 24873900
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 11. Strategic thinking in the shadow of self-enhancement: Benefits and costs.
    Grüning DJ; Krueger JI
    Br J Soc Psychol; 2024 Oct; 63(4):1725-1742. PubMed ID: 38634756
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 12. Strong Stackelberg reasoning in symmetric games: An experimental replication and extension.
    Pulford BD; Colman AM; Lawrence CL
    PeerJ; 2014; 2():e263. PubMed ID: 24688846
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 13. Learning by playing: A cross-sectional descriptive study of nursing students' experiences of learning clinical reasoning.
    Koivisto JM; Multisilta J; Niemi H; Katajisto J; Eriksson E
    Nurse Educ Today; 2016 Oct; 45():22-8. PubMed ID: 27429399
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 14. Dynamic probability of reinforcement for cooperation: Random game termination in the centipede game.
    Krockow EM; Colman AM; Pulford BD
    J Exp Anal Behav; 2018 Mar; 109(2):349-364. PubMed ID: 29574760
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 15. Collective rationality in interactive decisions: evidence for team reasoning.
    Colman AM; Pulford BD; Rose J
    Acta Psychol (Amst); 2008 Jun; 128(2):387-97. PubMed ID: 17868630
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 16. Representation effects in the centipede game.
    Crosetto P; Mantovani M
    PLoS One; 2018; 13(10):e0204422. PubMed ID: 30286092
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 17. Experimental Evidence on Iterated Reasoning in Games.
    Grehl S; Tutić A
    PLoS One; 2015; 10(8):e0136524. PubMed ID: 26312486
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 18. [Decisions in conditional situation and theory of mind in schizotypy].
    Monestes JL; Villatte M; Moore A; Yon V; Loas G
    Encephale; 2008 Apr; 34(2):116-22. PubMed ID: 18597718
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 19. Prosocial behavior in aging: which factors can explain age-related differences in social-economic decision making?
    Rosi A; Nola M; Lecce S; Cavallini E
    Int Psychogeriatr; 2019 Dec; 31(12):1747-1757. PubMed ID: 30782235
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 20. Quantum-mechanical machinery for rational decision-making in classical guessing game.
    Bang J; Ryu J; Pawłowski M; Ham BS; Lee J
    Sci Rep; 2016 Feb; 6():21424. PubMed ID: 26875685
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Next]    [New Search]
    of 10.