These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

148 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 23033743)

  • 1. Further tests of pigeons' (Columba livia) planning behavior using a computerized plus-shaped maze task.
    Miyata H; Fujita K
    Percept Mot Skills; 2012 Aug; 115(1):27-42. PubMed ID: 23033743
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 2. Pigeons (Columba livia) plan future moves on computerized maze tasks.
    Miyata H; Fujita K
    Anim Cogn; 2008 Jul; 11(3):505-16. PubMed ID: 18256862
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 3. Flexible route selection by pigeons (Columba livia) on a computerized multi-goal navigation task with and without an "obstacle".
    Miyata H; Fujita K
    J Comp Psychol; 2011 Nov; 125(4):431-435. PubMed ID: 21707140
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 4. Route selection by pigeons (Columba livia) in "traveling salesperson" navigation tasks presented on an LCD screen.
    Miyata H; Fujita K
    J Comp Psychol; 2010 Nov; 124(4):433-46. PubMed ID: 20836595
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 5. Performance of pigeons (Columba livia) on maze problems presented on the LCD screen: in search for preplanning ability in an avian species.
    Miyata H; Ushitani T; Adachi I; Fujita K
    J Comp Psychol; 2006 Nov; 120(4):358-66. PubMed ID: 17115856
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 6. Pigeon (Columba livia) and rat (Rattus norvegicus) performance in the midsession reversal procedure depends upon cue dimensionality.
    McMillan N; Kirk CR; Roberts WA
    J Comp Psychol; 2014 Nov; 128(4):357-66. PubMed ID: 25111628
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 7. Pigeon (Columba livia) encoding of a goal location: the relative importance of shape geometry and slope information.
    Nardi D; Bingman VP
    J Comp Psychol; 2009 May; 123(2):204-16. PubMed ID: 19450027
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 8. Two successive neurocognitive processes captured by near-infrared spectroscopy: prefrontal activation during a computerized plus-shaped maze task.
    Miyata H; Watanabe S; Minagawa-Kawai Y
    Brain Res; 2011 Feb; 1374():90-9. PubMed ID: 21172310
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 9. Acquisition of a same-different discrimination task by pigeons (Columba livia).
    Miyata H; Fujita K
    Psychol Rep; 2012 Feb; 110(1):251-62. PubMed ID: 22489391
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 10. Flexible learning and use of multiple-landmark information by pigeons (Columba livia) in a touch screen-based goal searching task.
    Ushitani T; Jitsumori M
    J Comp Psychol; 2011 Aug; 125(3):317-27. PubMed ID: 21574692
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 11. Do pigeons (Columba livia) study for a test?
    Roberts WA; Feeney MC; McMillan N; MacPherson K; Musolino E; Petter M
    J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process; 2009 Apr; 35(2):129-42. PubMed ID: 19364222
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 12. Social learning and innovation are positively correlated in pigeons (Columba livia).
    Bouchard J; Goodyer W; Lefebvre L
    Anim Cogn; 2007 Apr; 10(2):259-66. PubMed ID: 17205290
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 13. Visually guided capture of a moving stimulus by the pigeon (Columba livia).
    Wilkinson A; Kirkpatrick K
    Anim Cogn; 2009 Jan; 12(1):127-44. PubMed ID: 18719954
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 14. Planning in human children (Homo sapiens) assessed by maze problems on the touch screen.
    Miyata H; Itakura S; Fujita K
    J Comp Psychol; 2009 Feb; 123(1):69-78. PubMed ID: 19236146
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 15. The role of nidopallium caudolaterale in the goal-directed behavior of pigeons.
    Liu X; Wan H; Li S; Shang Z; Shi L
    Behav Brain Res; 2017 May; 326():112-120. PubMed ID: 28288807
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 16. Further analysis of perception of reversed Müller-Lyer figures for pigeons (Columba livia).
    Nakamura N; Watanabe S; Fujita K
    Percept Mot Skills; 2009 Feb; 108(1):239-50. PubMed ID: 19425465
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 17. Pigeons' (Columba livia) hierarchical organization of local and global cues in touch screen tasks.
    Legge EL; Spetch ML; Batty ER
    Behav Processes; 2009 Feb; 80(2):128-39. PubMed ID: 19022355
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 18. Septal area lesions impair spatial working memory in homing pigeons (Columba livia).
    Peterson RM; Bingman VP
    Neurobiol Learn Mem; 2011 Sep; 96(2):353-60. PubMed ID: 21742046
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 19. Searching in the center: pigeons (Columba livid) encode relative distance from walls of an enclosure.
    Gray ER; Spetch ML; Kelly DM; Nguyen A
    J Comp Psychol; 2004 Mar; 118(1):113-7. PubMed ID: 15008679
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 20. Further analysis of perception of the standard Müller-Lyer figures in pigeons (Columba livia) and humans (Homo sapiens): effects of length of brackets.
    Nakamura N; Watanabe S; Fujita K
    J Comp Psychol; 2009 Aug; 123(3):287-94. PubMed ID: 19685970
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Next]    [New Search]
    of 8.