These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

108 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 23138936)

  • 41. Dogs (Canis familiaris) account for body orientation but not visual barriers when responding to pointing gestures.
    MacLean EL; Krupenye C; Hare B
    J Comp Psychol; 2014 Aug; 128(3):285-97. PubMed ID: 24611643
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 42. A prelinguistic gestural universal of human communication.
    Liszkowski U; Brown P; Callaghan T; Takada A; de Vos C
    Cogn Sci; 2012; 36(4):698-713. PubMed ID: 22303868
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 43. The response of guide dogs and pet dogs (Canis familiaris) to cues of human referential communication (pointing and gaze).
    Ittyerah M; Gaunet F
    Anim Cogn; 2009 Mar; 12(2):257-65. PubMed ID: 18769948
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 44. A comparative study of the use of visual communicative signals in interactions between dogs (Canis familiaris) and humans and cats (Felis catus) and humans.
    Miklósi A; Pongrácz P; Lakatos G; Topál J; Csányi V
    J Comp Psychol; 2005 May; 119(2):179-86. PubMed ID: 15982161
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 45. Gaze-following behind barriers in domestic dogs.
    Met A; Miklósi Á; Lakatos G
    Anim Cogn; 2014 Nov; 17(6):1401-5. PubMed ID: 24816625
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 46. Communication between domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) and humans: dogs are good learners.
    Elgier AM; Jakovcevic A; Barrera G; Mustaca AE; Bentosela M
    Behav Processes; 2009 Jul; 81(3):402-8. PubMed ID: 19520240
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 47. A nonverbal test of knowledge attribution: a comparative study on dogs and children.
    Virányi Z; Topál J; Miklósi A; Csányi V
    Anim Cogn; 2006 Jan; 9(1):13-26. PubMed ID: 15895261
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 48. Apes' and children's understanding of cooperative and competitive motives in a communicative situation.
    Herrmann E; Tomasello M
    Dev Sci; 2006 Sep; 9(5):518-29. PubMed ID: 16911454
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 49. Do owners have a clever hans effect on dogs? Results of a pointing study.
    Schmidjell T; Range F; Huber L; Virányi Z
    Front Psychol; 2012; 3():558. PubMed ID: 23272000
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 50. Infant pointing: communication to cooperate or communication to learn?
    Southgate V; van Maanen C; Csibra G
    Child Dev; 2007; 78(3):735-40. PubMed ID: 17517000
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 51. Human-like social skills in dogs?
    Hare B; Tomasello M
    Trends Cogn Sci; 2005 Sep; 9(9):439-44. PubMed ID: 16061417
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 52. Comprehension of signs by dolphins (Tursiops truncatus).
    Tschudin A; Call J; Dunbar RI; Harris G; van der Elst C
    J Comp Psychol; 2001 Mar; 115(1):100-5. PubMed ID: 11334212
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 53. Twelve-month-olds communicate helpfully and appropriately for knowledgeable and ignorant partners.
    Liszkowski U; Carpenter M; Tomasello M
    Cognition; 2008 Sep; 108(3):732-9. PubMed ID: 18721918
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 54. Physical size matters in the domestic dog's (Canis lupus familiaris) ability to use human pointing cues.
    Helton WS; Helton ND
    Behav Processes; 2010 Sep; 85(1):77-9. PubMed ID: 20553825
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 55. Domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) flexibly adjust their human-directed behavior to the actions of their human partners in a problem situation.
    Horn L; Virányi Z; Miklósi A; Huber L; Range F
    Anim Cogn; 2012 Jan; 15(1):57-71. PubMed ID: 21739136
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 56. Dogs (Canis familiaris) learn from their owners via observation in a manipulation task.
    Kubinyi E; Topál J; Miklósi A; Csányi V
    J Comp Psychol; 2003 Jun; 117(2):156-65. PubMed ID: 12856786
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 57. Use of experimenter-given cues in dogs.
    Miklösi A; Polgárdi R; Topál J; Csányi V
    Anim Cogn; 1998 Oct; 1(2):113-21. PubMed ID: 24399275
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 58. Reproducing human actions and action sequences: "Do as I Do!" in a dog.
    Topál J; Byrne RW; Miklósi A; Csányi V
    Anim Cogn; 2006 Oct; 9(4):355-67. PubMed ID: 17024511
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 59. Gestural communication in deaf children: the effects and noneffects of parental input on early language development.
    Goldin-Meadow S; Mylander C
    Monogr Soc Res Child Dev; 1984; 49(3-4):1-151. PubMed ID: 6537463
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 60. One-year-olds comprehend the communicative intentions behind gestures in a hiding game.
    Behne T; Carpenter M; Tomasello M
    Dev Sci; 2005 Nov; 8(6):492-9. PubMed ID: 16246240
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Previous]   [Next]    [New Search]
    of 6.