These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

164 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 23142036)

  • 1. Redundancy matters: flexible learning of multiple contingencies in infants.
    Sloutsky VM; Robinson CW
    Cognition; 2013 Feb; 126(2):156-64. PubMed ID: 23142036
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 2. Experience matters: 11-month-old infants can learn to use material information to predict the weight of novel objects.
    Hauf P; Paulus M
    Infant Behav Dev; 2011 Jun; 34(3):467-71. PubMed ID: 21665284
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 3. How 7-month-olds interpret ambiguous motion events: category-based reasoning in infancy.
    Pauen S; Träuble B
    Cogn Psychol; 2009 Nov; 59(3):275-95. PubMed ID: 19596267
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 4. Categorization of real and replica objects by 14- and 18-month-old infants.
    Arterberry ME; Bornstein MH
    Infant Behav Dev; 2012 Jun; 35(3):606-12. PubMed ID: 22742988
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 5. The nature and processes of preverbal learning: implications from nine-month-old infants' discrimination problem solving.
    Coldren JT; Colombo J
    Monogr Soc Res Child Dev; 1994; 59(4):1-75; discussion 76-93. PubMed ID: 7997251
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 6. Attentional learning and flexible induction: how mundane mechanisms give rise to smart behaviors.
    Sloutsky VM; Fisher AV
    Child Dev; 2008; 79(3):639-51. PubMed ID: 18489418
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 7. Learning words' sounds before learning how words sound: 9-month-olds use distinct objects as cues to categorize speech information.
    Yeung HH; Werker JF
    Cognition; 2009 Nov; 113(2):234-43. PubMed ID: 19765698
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 8. Toddlers can adaptively change how they categorize: same objects, same session, two different categorical distinctions.
    Horst JS; Ellis AE; Samuelson LK; Trejo E; Worzalla SL; Peltan JR; Oakes LM
    Dev Sci; 2009 Jan; 12(1):96-105. PubMed ID: 19120417
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 9. The perceptual effects of learning object categories that predict perceptual goals.
    Van Gulick AE; Gauthier I
    J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn; 2014 Sep; 40(5):1307-20. PubMed ID: 24820671
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 10. Categorization in infancy: labeling induces a persisting focus on commonalities.
    Althaus N; Plunkett K
    Dev Sci; 2016 Sep; 19(5):770-80. PubMed ID: 26538010
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 11. Specifying the role of function in infant categorization.
    Booth AE; Schuler K; Zajicek R
    Infant Behav Dev; 2010 Dec; 33(4):672-84. PubMed ID: 20951437
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 12. Modeling the effect of mood on dimensional attention during categorization.
    Zivot MT; Cohen AL; Kapucu A
    Emotion; 2013 Aug; 13(4):703-10. PubMed ID: 23627722
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 13. Nonverbal generics: human infants interpret objects as symbols of object kinds.
    Csibra G; Shamsudheen R
    Annu Rev Psychol; 2015 Jan; 66():689-710. PubMed ID: 25251493
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 14. Neural markers of categorization in 6-month-old infants.
    Quinn PC; Westerlund A; Nelson CA
    Psychol Sci; 2006 Jan; 17(1):59-66. PubMed ID: 16371145
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 15. Extremely selective attention: eye-tracking studies of the dynamic allocation of attention to stimulus features in categorization.
    Blair MR; Watson MR; Walshe RC; Maj F
    J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn; 2009 Sep; 35(5):1196-206. PubMed ID: 19686015
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 16. Just do it? Investigating the gap between prediction and action in toddlers' causal inferences.
    Bonawitz EB; Ferranti D; Saxe R; Gopnik A; Meltzoff AN; Woodward J; Schulz LE
    Cognition; 2010 Apr; 115(1):104-17. PubMed ID: 20097329
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 17. Flexible and context-dependent categorization by eighteen-month-olds.
    Mareschal D; Tan SH
    Child Dev; 2007; 78(1):19-37. PubMed ID: 17328691
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 18. Infants flexibly use different dimensions to categorize objects.
    Ellis AE; Oakes LM
    Dev Psychol; 2006 Nov; 42(6):1000-11. PubMed ID: 17087536
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 19. Learning to Individuate: The Specificity of Labels Differentially Impacts Infant Visual Attention.
    Pickron CB; Iyer A; Fava E; Scott LS
    Child Dev; 2018 May; 89(3):698-710. PubMed ID: 29219170
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 20. Context modulation of learned attention deployment.
    Uengoer M; Pearce JM; Lachnit H; Koenig S
    Learn Behav; 2018 Mar; 46(1):23-37. PubMed ID: 28597217
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Next]    [New Search]
    of 9.