These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

234 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 23205408)

  • 21. Pulling out the intentional structure of action: the relation between action processing and action production in infancy.
    Sommerville JA; Woodward AL
    Cognition; 2005 Feb; 95(1):1-30. PubMed ID: 15629472
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 22. Sensitivity to communicative relevance tells young children what to imitate.
    Southgate V; Chevallier C; Csibra G
    Dev Sci; 2009 Nov; 12(6):1013-9. PubMed ID: 19840055
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 23. A self-agency bias in preschoolers' causal inferences.
    Kushnir T; Wellman HM; Gelman SA
    Dev Psychol; 2009 Mar; 45(2):597-603. PubMed ID: 19271843
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 24. Biases towards internal features in infants' reasoning about objects.
    Newman GE; Herrmann P; Wynn K; Keil FC
    Cognition; 2008 May; 107(2):420-32. PubMed ID: 18068151
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 25. Nonverbal components of Theory of Mind in typical and atypical development.
    Kampis D; Fogd D; Kovács ÁM
    Infant Behav Dev; 2017 Aug; 48(Pt A):54-62. PubMed ID: 27856031
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 26. The influence of a bystander agent's beliefs on children's and adults' decision-making process.
    Buttelmann F; Buttelmann D
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2017 Jan; 153():126-139. PubMed ID: 27741442
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 27. Communication in action: Planning and interpreting communicative demonstrations.
    Ho MK; Cushman F; Littman ML; Austerweil JL
    J Exp Psychol Gen; 2021 Nov; 150(11):2246-2272. PubMed ID: 34498911
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 28. Social cognition, joint attention, and communicative competence from 9 to 15 months of age.
    Carpenter M; Nagell K; Tomasello M
    Monogr Soc Res Child Dev; 1998; 63(4):i-vi, 1-143. PubMed ID: 9835078
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 29. Communicating shared knowledge in infancy.
    Egyed K; Király I; Gergely G
    Psychol Sci; 2013 Jul; 24(7):1348-53. PubMed ID: 23719664
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 30. Eighteen-month-olds understand false beliefs in an unexpected-contents task.
    Buttelmann D; Over H; Carpenter M; Tomasello M
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2014 Mar; 119():120-6. PubMed ID: 24267393
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 31. Infants' use of contextual cues in the generalization of effective actions from imitation.
    Yang DJ; Bushnell EW; Buchanan DW; Sobel DM
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2013 Oct; 116(2):510-31. PubMed ID: 23357091
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 32. Précis of bayesian rationality: The probabilistic approach to human reasoning.
    Oaksford M; Chater N
    Behav Brain Sci; 2009 Feb; 32(1):69-84; discussion 85-120. PubMed ID: 19210833
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 33. No evidence for adult smartphone use affecting attribution of communicative intention in toddlers: Online imitation study using the Sock Ball Task.
    Flatebø S; Óturai G; Hernik M
    PLoS One; 2024; 19(3):e0300874. PubMed ID: 38517933
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 34. Where will the triangle look for it? Attributing false beliefs to a geometric shape at 17 months.
    Surian L; Geraci A
    Br J Dev Psychol; 2012 Mar; 30(Pt 1):30-44. PubMed ID: 22429031
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 35. Examining functional mechanisms of imitative learning in infancy: does teleological reasoning affect infants' imitation beyond motor resonance?
    Paulus M; Hunnius S; Bekkering H
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2013 Oct; 116(2):487-98. PubMed ID: 23270792
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 36. Domain generality and specificity in children's causal inference about ambiguous data.
    Sobel DM; Munro SE
    Dev Psychol; 2009 Mar; 45(2):511-24. PubMed ID: 19271835
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 37. Surprise! 20-month-old infants understand the emotional consequences of false beliefs.
    Scott RM
    Cognition; 2017 Feb; 159():33-47. PubMed ID: 27886520
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 38. Infants differentiate between successful and failed communication in a false-belief context.
    Schulze C; Buttelmann D
    Infant Behav Dev; 2022 Nov; 69():101770. PubMed ID: 36113367
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 39. The role of social signals in segmenting observed actions in 18-month-old children.
    Kliesch C; Parise E; Reid V; Hoehl S
    Dev Sci; 2022 May; 25(3):e13198. PubMed ID: 34820963
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 40. A Bayesian framework for the development of belief-desire reasoning: Estimating inhibitory power.
    Wang L; Hemmer P; Leslie AM
    Psychon Bull Rev; 2019 Feb; 26(1):205-221. PubMed ID: 30030716
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Previous]   [Next]    [New Search]
    of 12.