These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

188 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 23251395)

  • 1. Is the cultural transmission of irrelevant tool actions in adult humans (Homo sapiens) best explained as the result of an evolved conformist bias?
    McGuigan N; Gladstone D; Cook L
    PLoS One; 2012; 7(12):e50863. PubMed ID: 23251395
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 2. The influence of peers on the tendency of 3- and 4-year-old children to over-imitate.
    McGuigan N; Robertson S
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2015 Aug; 136():42-54. PubMed ID: 25897959
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 3. From over-imitation to super-copying: adults imitate causally irrelevant aspects of tool use with higher fidelity than young children.
    McGuigan N; Makinson J; Whiten A
    Br J Psychol; 2011 Feb; 102(1):1-18. PubMed ID: 21241282
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 4. Over-imitation is not automatic: context sensitivity in children's overimitation and action interpretation of causally irrelevant actions.
    Keupp S; Behne T; Zachow J; Kasbohm A; Rakoczy H
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2015 Feb; 130():163-75. PubMed ID: 25462039
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 5. Evidence for a dual-process account of over-imitation: Children imitate anti- and prosocial models equally, but prefer prosocial models once they become aware of multiple solutions to a task.
    Schleihauf H; Hoehl S
    PLoS One; 2021; 16(9):e0256614. PubMed ID: 34529702
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 6. Rational over-imitation: Preschoolers consider material costs and copy causally irrelevant actions selectively.
    Keupp S; Bancken C; Schillmöller J; Rakoczy H; Behne T
    Cognition; 2016 Feb; 147():85-92. PubMed ID: 26649758
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 7. A model's natural group membership affects over-imitation in 6-year-olds.
    Krieger AAR; Aschersleben G; Sommerfeld L; Buttelmann D
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2020 Apr; 192():104783. PubMed ID: 31951928
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 8. When does it pay to follow the crowd? Children optimize imitation of causally irrelevant actions performed by a majority.
    Evans CL; Burdett ERR; Murray K; Carpenter M
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2021 Dec; 212():105229. PubMed ID: 34284228
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 9. Causal knowledge and imitation/emulation switching in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and children (Homo sapiens).
    Horner V; Whiten A
    Anim Cogn; 2005 Jul; 8(3):164-81. PubMed ID: 15549502
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 10. Cultural differences in the imitation and transmission of inefficient actions.
    Corriveau KH; DiYanni CJ; Clegg JM; Min G; Chin J; Nasrini J
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2017 Sep; 161():1-18. PubMed ID: 28456052
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 11. Is the tendency to conform influenced by the age of the majority?
    McGuigan N; Burgess V
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2017 May; 157():49-65. PubMed ID: 28110153
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 12. The role of consensus and culture in children's imitation of inefficient actions.
    DiYanni CJ; Corriveau KH; Kurkul K; Nasrini J; Nini D
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2015 Sep; 137():99-110. PubMed ID: 25965007
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 13. The role of transmission biases in the cultural diffusion of irrelevant actions.
    McGuigan N
    J Comp Psychol; 2012 May; 126(2):150-60. PubMed ID: 21942570
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 14. Selective copying of the majority suggests children are broadly "optimal-" rather than "over-" imitators.
    Evans CL; Laland KN; Carpenter M; Kendal RL
    Dev Sci; 2018 Sep; 21(5):e12637. PubMed ID: 29250871
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 15. Children over-imitate adults and peers more than puppets.
    Stengelin R; Ball R; Maurits L; Kanngiesser P; Haun DBM
    Dev Sci; 2023 Mar; 26(2):e13303. PubMed ID: 35818836
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 16. Developmental changes in the influence of conventional and instrumental cues on over-imitation in 3- to 6-year-old children.
    Moraru CA; Gomez JC; McGuigan N
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2016 May; 145():34-47. PubMed ID: 26774258
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 17. "Model age-based" and "copy when uncertain" biases in children's social learning of a novel task.
    Wood LA; Harrison RA; Lucas AJ; McGuigan N; Burdett ERR; Whiten A
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2016 Oct; 150():272-284. PubMed ID: 27371768
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 18. Emulation and "overemulation" in the social learning of causally opaque versus causally transparent tool use by 23- and 30-month-olds.
    McGuigan N; Whiten A
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2009 Dec; 104(4):367-81. PubMed ID: 19683722
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 19. The influence of model status on the tendency of young children to over-imitate.
    McGuigan N
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2013 Dec; 116(4):962-9. PubMed ID: 23768760
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 20. Irrelevant-action imitation is short-term and contextual: Evidence from two under-studied populations.
    Kline MA; Gervais MM; Moya C; Boyd RT
    Dev Sci; 2020 May; 23(3):e12903. PubMed ID: 31505090
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Next]    [New Search]
    of 10.