These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

147 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 23335836)

  • 21. Children's (and Adults') Production Adjustments to Generic and Particular Listener Needs.
    Grigoroglou M; Papafragou A
    Cogn Sci; 2019 Oct; 43(10):e12790. PubMed ID: 31621121
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 22. The impact of generic language about ability on children's achievement motivation.
    Cimpian A
    Dev Psychol; 2010 Sep; 46(5):1333-40. PubMed ID: 20822242
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 23. Why are dunkels sticky? Preschoolers infer functionality and intentional creation for artifact properties learned from generic language.
    Cimpian A; Cadena C
    Cognition; 2010 Oct; 117(1):62-8. PubMed ID: 20656283
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 24. Coherence in children's speech perception.
    Nittrouer S; Crowther CS
    J Acoust Soc Am; 2001 Oct; 110(4):2129-40. PubMed ID: 11681390
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 25. The influence of labels and facts on children's and adults' categorization.
    Johanson M; Papafragou A
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2016 Apr; 144():130-51. PubMed ID: 26735976
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 26. Determining that a label is kind-referring: factors that influence children's and adults' novel word extensions.
    Tare M; Gelman SA
    J Child Lang; 2010 Nov; 37(5):1007-26. PubMed ID: 19874640
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 27. What do children want to know about animals and artifacts? Domain-specific requests for information.
    Greif ML; Kemler Nelson DG; Keil FC; Gutierrez F
    Psychol Sci; 2006 Jun; 17(6):455-9. PubMed ID: 16771792
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 28. When humans become animals: Development of the animal category in early childhood.
    Herrmann PA; Medin DL; Waxman SR
    Cognition; 2012 Jan; 122(1):74-9. PubMed ID: 21944836
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 29. One frog, two frog, red frog, blue frog: factors affecting children's syntactic choices in production and comprehension.
    Hurewitz F; Brown-Schmidt S; Thorpe K; Gleitman LR; Trueswell JC
    J Psycholinguist Res; 2000 Nov; 29(6):597-626. PubMed ID: 11196065
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 30. Generic language and judgements about category membership: Can generics highlight properties as central?
    Hollander MA; Gelman SA; Raman L
    Lang Cogn Process; 2009 May; 24(4):481-505. PubMed ID: 25620828
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 31. Knowledge, expectations, and inductive reasoning within conceptual hierarchies.
    Coley JD; Hayes B; Lawson C; Moloney M
    Cognition; 2004 Jan; 90(3):217-53. PubMed ID: 14667696
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 32. Grammatical Word Production Across Metrical Contexts in School-Aged Children's and Adults' Speech.
    Redford MA
    J Speech Lang Hear Res; 2018 Jun; 61(6):1339-1354. PubMed ID: 29800072
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 33. A developmental intergroup theory of social stereotypes and prejudice.
    Bigler RS; Liben LS
    Adv Child Dev Behav; 2006; 34():39-89. PubMed ID: 17120802
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 34. Figuring out function: Children's and adults' use of ownership information in judgments of artifact function.
    Banerjee K; Kominsky JF; Fernando M; Keil FC
    Dev Psychol; 2015 Dec; 51(12):1791-801. PubMed ID: 26414095
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 35. Two-year-olds use the generic/nongeneric distinction to guide their inferences about novel kinds.
    Graham SA; Nayer SL; Gelman SA
    Child Dev; 2011; 82(2):493-507. PubMed ID: 21410928
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 36. The emergence of semantic categorization in early visual processing: ERP indices of animal vs. artifact recognition.
    Proverbio AM; Del Zotto M; Zani A
    BMC Neurosci; 2007 Apr; 8():24. PubMed ID: 17411424
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 37. Children's and adults' perception of questions and statements from terminal fundamental frequency contours.
    Saindon MR; Cirelli LK; Schellenberg EG; van Lieshout P; Trehub SE
    J Acoust Soc Am; 2017 May; 141(5):3123. PubMed ID: 28599538
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 38. Young children's knowledge of the "determiner" and "adjective" categories.
    Kemp N; Lieven E; Tomasello M
    J Speech Lang Hear Res; 2005 Jun; 48(3):592-609. PubMed ID: 16197275
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 39. Five-Year-Olds' and Adults' Use of Paralinguistic Cues to Overcome Referential Uncertainty.
    Thacker JM; Chambers CG; Graham SA
    Front Psychol; 2018; 9():143. PubMed ID: 29487559
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 40. Fishy fishes: the typicality of object stimuli used to assess children's language in the Reynell Development Language Scales--III.
    Syrnyk C; Meints K
    Int J Lang Commun Disord; 2012; 47(4):437-50. PubMed ID: 22788229
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Previous]   [Next]    [New Search]
    of 8.