These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

141 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 23646001)

  • 1. Oculomotor Exploration of Impossible Figures in Early Infancy.
    Shuwairi SM; Johnson SP
    Infancy; 2013; 18(2):221-232. PubMed ID: 23646001
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 2. Increased visual interest and affective responses to impossible figures in early infancy.
    Krause C; Longo D; Shuwairi S
    Infant Behav Dev; 2019 Nov; 57():101341. PubMed ID: 31382134
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 3. Preference for impossible figures in 4-month-olds.
    Shuwairi SM
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2009 Sep; 104(1):115-23. PubMed ID: 19038399
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 4. Discrimination of possible and impossible objects in infancy.
    Shuwairi SM; Albert MK; Johnson SP
    Psychol Sci; 2007 Apr; 18(4):303-7. PubMed ID: 17470252
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 5. Infants' Response to Pictures of Impossible Objects.
    Shuwairi SM; Tran A; DeLoache JS; Johnson SP
    Infancy; 2010 Nov; 15(6):636-649. PubMed ID: 32693461
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 6. Holistic processing of impossible objects: evidence from Garner's speeded-classification task.
    Freud E; Avidan G; Ganel T
    Vision Res; 2013 Dec; 93():10-8. PubMed ID: 24121070
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 7. The developmental origins of naïve psychology in infancy.
    Poulin-Dubois D; Brooker I; Chow V
    Adv Child Dev Behav; 2009; 37():55-104. PubMed ID: 19673160
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 8. The development of the ability of infants to utilize static cues to create and access representations of object shape.
    Tsuruhara A; Sawada T; Kanazawa S; Yamaguchi MK; Corrow S; Yonas A
    J Vis; 2010 Oct; 10(12):2. PubMed ID: 21047734
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 9. Measuring young infants' sensitivity to height-in-the-picture-plane by contrasting monocular and binocular preferential-looking.
    Tsuruhara A; Corrow S; Kanazawa S; Yamaguchi MK; Yonas A
    Dev Psychobiol; 2014 Jan; 56(1):109-16. PubMed ID: 23280555
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 10. Infants' sensitivity to pictorial depth cues: a review and meta-analysis of looking studies.
    Kavšek M; Yonas A; Granrud CE
    Infant Behav Dev; 2012 Feb; 35(1):109-28. PubMed ID: 21893347
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 11. Infant's ability to form a common representation of an object's shape from different pictorial depth cues: A transfer-across-cues study.
    Tsuruhara A; Sawada T; Kanazawa S; Yamaguchi MK; Yonas A
    Infant Behav Dev; 2009 Dec; 32(4):468-75. PubMed ID: 19801169
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 12. Evidence for similar early but not late representation of possible and impossible objects.
    Freud E; Hadad BS; Avidan G; Ganel T
    Front Psychol; 2015; 6():94. PubMed ID: 25762949
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 13. Learning to see the impossible.
    Young AW; Deregowski JB
    Perception; 1981; 10(1):91-105. PubMed ID: 7255087
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 14. Object-based visual attention in 8-month-old infants: evidence from an eye-tracking study.
    Bulf H; Valenza E
    Dev Psychol; 2013 Oct; 49(10):1909-18. PubMed ID: 23276133
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 15. Young infants' perception of the trajectories of two- and three-dimensional objects.
    Johnson SP; Gavin Bremner J; Slater AM; Shuwairi SM; Mason U; Spring J; Usherwood B
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2012 Sep; 113(1):177-85. PubMed ID: 22704037
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 16. Infants' perceptions of constraints on object motion as a function of object shape.
    Jowkar-Baniani G; Paolozza A; Greene A; Cheng CK; Schmuckler MA
    Cognition; 2017 Aug; 165():126-136. PubMed ID: 28538162
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 17. Lower region: a new cue for figure-ground assignment.
    Vecera SP; Vogel EK; Woodman GF
    J Exp Psychol Gen; 2002 Jun; 131(2):194-205. PubMed ID: 12049239
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 18. Spontaneous discrimination of possible and impossible objects by newly hatched chicks.
    Regolin L; Rugani R; Stancher G; Vallortigara G
    Biol Lett; 2011 Oct; 7(5):654-7. PubMed ID: 21429912
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 19. Pictorial cues and three-dimensional information processing in early infancy.
    Bhatt RS; Bertin E
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2001 Dec; 80(4):315-32. PubMed ID: 11689033
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 20. The role of eye movements in depth from motion parallax during infancy.
    Nawrot E; Nawrot M
    J Vis; 2013 Dec; 13(14):. PubMed ID: 24353309
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Next]    [New Search]
    of 8.