These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.
4. Monocular and binocular edges enhance the perception of stereoscopic slant. Wardle SG; Palmisano S; Gillam BJ Vision Res; 2014 Jul; 100():113-23. PubMed ID: 24802594 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
5. Illusory depth perception of oblique lines produced by overlaid vertical disparity. Ito H Vision Res; 2005 Mar; 45(7):931-42. PubMed ID: 15644231 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
6. Stereoscopic Depth Contrast in a 3D Müller-Lyer Configuration: Evidence for Local Normalization. Harada S; Mitsudo H Perception; 2017 Jul; 46(7):860-873. PubMed ID: 28073303 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
7. A physiological theory of depth perception from vertical disparity. Matthews N; Meng X; Xu P; Qian N Vision Res; 2003 Jan; 43(1):85-99. PubMed ID: 12505608 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
8. Anisotropies in the perception of stereoscopic surfaces: the role of orientation disparity. Cagenello R; Rogers BJ Vision Res; 1993 Nov; 33(16):2189-201. PubMed ID: 8273286 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
9. Selective biasing of stereo correspondence in an ambiguous stereogram. Goutcher R; Mamassian P Vision Res; 2005 Feb; 45(4):469-83. PubMed ID: 15610751 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
10. Recovering 3-D shape: roles of absolute and relative disparity, retinal size, and viewing distance as studied with reverse-perspective stimuli. Dobias JJ; Papathomas TV Perception; 2013; 42(4):430-46. PubMed ID: 23866556 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
11. The perceived visual direction of monocular objects in random-dot stereograms is influenced by perceived depth and allelotropia. Hariharan-Vilupuru S; Bedell HE Vision Res; 2009 Jan; 49(2):190-201. PubMed ID: 18992271 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
12. Spatial frequency integration for binocular correspondence in macaque area V4. Kumano H; Tanabe S; Fujita I J Neurophysiol; 2008 Jan; 99(1):402-8. PubMed ID: 17959744 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
13. Eye movements facilitate stereo-slant discrimination when horizontal disparity is noisy. Berends EM; Zhang ZL; Schor CM J Vis; 2003 Dec; 3(11):780-94. PubMed ID: 14765961 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
14. Depth aftereffects mediated by vertical disparities: evidence for vertical disparity driven calibration of extraretinal signals during stereopsis. Duke PA; Oruç I; Qi H; Backus BT Vision Res; 2006 Jan; 46(1-2):228-41. PubMed ID: 16257031 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
15. Binocular perception of slant about oblique axes relative to a visual frame of reference. van Ee R; Erkelens CJ Perception; 1995; 24(3):299-314. PubMed ID: 7617431 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
16. Relative size disparities and the perception of surface slant. Kaneko H; Howard IP Vision Res; 1996 Jul; 36(13):1919-30. PubMed ID: 8759431 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
17. Stereo correspondence is optimized for large viewing distances. Phillipson GP; Read JC Eur J Neurosci; 2010 Dec; 32(11):1959-69. PubMed ID: 20955471 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
19. Slant or occlusion: global factors resolve stereoscopic ambiguity in sets of horizontal lines. Gillam B; Grove PM Vision Res; 2004; 44(20):2359-66. PubMed ID: 15246752 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
20. Vertical-disparity gradients are processed independently in different depth planes. Duke PA; Howard IP Vision Res; 2005 Jul; 45(15):2025-35. PubMed ID: 15820519 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related] [Next] [New Search]