These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.
24. The Development of Infants' Sensitivity to Behavioral Intentions when Inferring Others' Social Preferences. Lee YE; Yun JE; Kim EY; Song HJ PLoS One; 2015; 10(9):e0135588. PubMed ID: 26383160 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
25. Perception and production of object-related grasping in 6-month-olds. Daum MM; Prinz W; Aschersleben G J Exp Child Psychol; 2011 Apr; 108(4):810-8. PubMed ID: 21092981 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
26. Just rewards: 17-Month-old infants expect agents to take resources according to the principles of distributive justice. Wang Y; Henderson AME J Exp Child Psychol; 2018 Aug; 172():25-40. PubMed ID: 29574235 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
27. Infants understand the referential nature of human gaze but not robot gaze. Okumura Y; Kanakogi Y; Kanda T; Ishiguro H; Itakura S J Exp Child Psychol; 2013 Sep; 116(1):86-95. PubMed ID: 23660178 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
28. Infants' attentional preference for object-related actions of older children compared to object-related actions of peers and adults. Zmyj N; Daum MM; Prinz W; Aschersleben G Infant Behav Dev; 2012 Jun; 35(3):533-42. PubMed ID: 22721750 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
29. Human infants' understanding of social imitation: Inferences of affiliation from third party observations. Powell LJ; Spelke ES Cognition; 2018 Jan; 170():31-48. PubMed ID: 28938173 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
31. Learning to walk changes infants' social interactions. Clearfield MW Infant Behav Dev; 2011 Feb; 34(1):15-25. PubMed ID: 20478619 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
32. Being mimicked increases prosocial behavior in 18-month-old infants. Carpenter M; Uebel J; Tomasello M Child Dev; 2013; 84(5):1511-8. PubMed ID: 23488734 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
33. Friends, friendlessness, and social cognition. Wellman HM Br J Dev Psychol; 2015 Mar; 33(1):24-6. PubMed ID: 25441369 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
34. Preverbal infants' reactions to third-party punishments and rewards delivered toward fair and unfair agents. Geraci A; Surian L J Exp Child Psychol; 2023 Feb; 226():105574. PubMed ID: 36332434 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
35. Infants' individuation of agents and inert objects. Surian L; Caldi S Dev Sci; 2010 Jan; 13(1):143-50. PubMed ID: 20121870 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
36. Toddlers map the word 'good' to helping agents, but not to fair distributors. Franchin L; Savazzi F; Neira-Gutierrez IC; Surian L J Child Lang; 2019 Jan; 46(1):98-110. PubMed ID: 30180913 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
37. Infants' understanding of intention from 10 to 14 months: Interrelations among violation of expectancy and imitation tasks. Olineck KM; Poulin-Dubois D Infant Behav Dev; 2009 Dec; 32(4):404-15. PubMed ID: 19619899 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
38. The competition of assessment rules for indirect reciprocity. Uchida S; Sigmund K J Theor Biol; 2010 Mar; 263(1):13-9. PubMed ID: 19962390 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
39. Evaluations of aggressive chasing interactions by 7-month-old infants. Geraci A; Benavides-Varela S; Nascimben C; Simion F; Di Giorgio E Aggress Behav; 2024 Aug; 50(5):e22174. PubMed ID: 39229968 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
40. Do infants and nonhuman animals attribute mental states? Burge T Psychol Rev; 2018 Apr; 125(3):409-434. PubMed ID: 29733666 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related] [Previous] [Next] [New Search]