These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

219 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 24045001)

  • 41. Memory for temporal order in action is slow developing, sensitive to deviant input, and supported by foundational cognitive processes.
    Loucks J; Price HL
    Dev Psychol; 2019 Feb; 55(2):263-273. PubMed ID: 30407022
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 42. Performance of motor imitation in children with and without dyspraxia.
    Ruttanathantong K; Siritaratiwat W; Sriphetcharawut S; Emasithi A; Saengsuwan J; Saengsuwan J
    J Med Assoc Thai; 2013 Jul; 96(7):794-800. PubMed ID: 24319849
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 43. Child-directed action promotes 2-year-olds' imitation.
    Williamson RA; Brand RJ
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2014 Feb; 118():119-26. PubMed ID: 24079611
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 44. Interactions between causal models, theories, and social cognitive development.
    Sobel DM; Buchanan DW; Butterfield J; Jenkins OC
    Neural Netw; 2010; 23(8-9):1060-71. PubMed ID: 20615661
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 45. Parameters of remembering and forgetting in the transition from infancy to early childhood.
    Bauer PJ; Wenner JA; Dropik PL; Wewerka SS
    Monogr Soc Res Child Dev; 2000; 65(4):i-vi, 1-204. PubMed ID: 12467092
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 46. Experimental "microcultures" in young children: identifying biographic, cognitive, and social predictors of information transmission.
    Flynn E; Whiten A
    Child Dev; 2012; 83(3):911-25. PubMed ID: 22417384
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 47. Imitation, pretend play, and childhood: essential elements in the evolution of human culture?
    Nielsen M
    J Comp Psychol; 2012 May; 126(2):170-81. PubMed ID: 21859186
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 48. The effect of personal relevance on psychological inference: a developmental analysis.
    Feldman NS; Ruble DN
    Child Dev; 1988 Oct; 59(5):1339-52. PubMed ID: 3168644
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 49. "Model age-based" and "copy when uncertain" biases in children's social learning of a novel task.
    Wood LA; Harrison RA; Lucas AJ; McGuigan N; Burdett ERR; Whiten A
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2016 Oct; 150():272-284. PubMed ID: 27371768
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 50. Two-year-olds are vigilant of others' non-verbal cues to credibility.
    Birch SA; Akmal N; Frampton KL
    Dev Sci; 2010 Mar; 13(2):363-9. PubMed ID: 20136933
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 51. From over-imitation to super-copying: adults imitate causally irrelevant aspects of tool use with higher fidelity than young children.
    McGuigan N; Makinson J; Whiten A
    Br J Psychol; 2011 Feb; 102(1):1-18. PubMed ID: 21241282
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 52. How self-generated labelling shapes transfer of learning during early childhood: The role of individual differences.
    Moser A; Olsen S; Rusnak SN; Barr R; Gerhardstein P
    Br J Dev Psychol; 2019 Mar; 37(1):68-83. PubMed ID: 29981173
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 53. Neonatal imitation in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) tested with two paradigms.
    Bard KA
    Anim Cogn; 2007 Apr; 10(2):233-42. PubMed ID: 17180698
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 54. "Should I or shouldn't I?" Imitation of undesired versus allowed actions from peer and adult models by 18- and 24-month-old toddlers.
    Seehagen S; Schneider S; Miebach K; Frigge K; Zmyj N
    Infant Behav Dev; 2017 Nov; 49():1-8. PubMed ID: 28646677
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 55. Do as I do, not as I say: Actions speak louder than words in preschoolers learning from others.
    Corriveau KH; Min G; Chin J; Doan S
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2016 Mar; 143():179-87. PubMed ID: 26610716
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 56. Children's synchrony and rhythmicity in imitation of peers: toward a developmental model of empathy.
    Xavier J; Tilmont E; Bonnot O
    J Physiol Paris; 2013 Sep; 107(4):291-7. PubMed ID: 23583461
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 57. Human infants' understanding of social imitation: Inferences of affiliation from third party observations.
    Powell LJ; Spelke ES
    Cognition; 2018 Jan; 170():31-48. PubMed ID: 28938173
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 58. Developmental change and the nature of learning in childhood.
    Ramscar M; Gitcho N
    Trends Cogn Sci; 2007 Jul; 11(7):274-9. PubMed ID: 17560161
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 59. The reliability of a model influences 14-month-olds' imitation.
    Zmyj N; Buttelmann D; Carpenter M; Daum MM
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2010 Aug; 106(4):208-20. PubMed ID: 20427052
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 60. A model's natural group membership affects over-imitation in 6-year-olds.
    Krieger AAR; Aschersleben G; Sommerfeld L; Buttelmann D
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2020 Apr; 192():104783. PubMed ID: 31951928
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Previous]   [Next]    [New Search]
    of 11.