These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

165 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 24120635)

  • 1. African elephants can use human pointing cues to find hidden food.
    Smet AF; Byrne RW
    Curr Biol; 2013 Oct; 23(20):2033-7. PubMed ID: 24120635
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 2. Interpretation of human pointing by African elephants: generalisation and rationality.
    Smet AF; Byrne RW
    Anim Cogn; 2014 Nov; 17(6):1365-74. PubMed ID: 24942107
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 3. African elephants interpret a trunk gesture as a clue to direction of interest.
    Smet AF; Byrne RW
    Curr Biol; 2020 Aug; 30(16):R926-R927. PubMed ID: 32810448
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 4. Sea lions' (Zalophus californianus) use of human pointing gestures as referential cues.
    Malassis R; Delfour F
    Learn Behav; 2015 Jun; 43(2):101-12. PubMed ID: 25678395
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 5. African elephants (Loxodonta africana) recognize visual attention from face and body orientation.
    Smet AF; Byrne RW
    Biol Lett; 2014 Jul; 10(7):. PubMed ID: 25013015
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 6. Visual cues given by humans are not sufficient for Asian elephants (Elephas maximus) to find hidden food.
    Plotnik JM; Pokorny JJ; Keratimanochaya T; Webb C; Beronja HF; Hennessy A; Hill J; Hill VJ; Kiss R; Maguire C; Melville BL; Morrison VM; Seecoomar D; Singer B; Ukehaxhaj J; Vlahakis SK; Ylli D; Clayton NS; Roberts J; Fure EL; Duchatelier AP; Getz D
    PLoS One; 2013; 8(4):e61174. PubMed ID: 23613804
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 7. The use of a human's location and social cues by Asian elephants in an object-choice task.
    Ketchaisri O; Siripunkaw C; Plotnik JM
    Anim Cogn; 2019 Nov; 22(6):907-915. PubMed ID: 31218577
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 8. Comprehension of human pointing gestures in young human-reared wolves (Canis lupus) and dogs (Canis familiaris).
    Virányi Z; Gácsi M; Kubinyi E; Topál J; Belényi B; Ujfalussy D; Miklósi A
    Anim Cogn; 2008 Jul; 11(3):373-87. PubMed ID: 18183437
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 9. Use of experimenter-given cues by African gray parrots (Psittacus erithacus).
    Giret N; Miklósi A; Kreutzer M; Bovet D
    Anim Cogn; 2009 Jan; 12(1):1-10. PubMed ID: 18543008
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 10. Olive baboons communicate intentionally by pointing.
    Meunier H; Prieur J; Vauclair J
    Anim Cogn; 2013 Mar; 16(2):155-63. PubMed ID: 22955704
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 11. Domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) use human gestures but not nonhuman tokens to find hidden food.
    Udell MA; Giglio RF; Wynne CD
    J Comp Psychol; 2008 Feb; 122(1):84-93. PubMed ID: 18298285
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 12. Dingoes (Canis dingo) can use human social cues to locate hidden food.
    Smith BP; Litchfield CA
    Anim Cogn; 2010 Mar; 13(2):367-76. PubMed ID: 19779743
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 13. Comprehension of the communicative intent behind pointing and gazing gestures by young children with Williams syndrome or Down syndrome.
    John AE; Mervis CB
    J Speech Lang Hear Res; 2010 Aug; 53(4):950-60. PubMed ID: 20605941
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 14. Comprehension of human pointing gestures in horses (Equus caballus).
    Maros K; Gácsi M; Miklósi A
    Anim Cogn; 2008 Jul; 11(3):457-66. PubMed ID: 18247069
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 15. Do dogs follow behavioral cues from an unreliable human?
    Takaoka A; Maeda T; Hori Y; Fujita K
    Anim Cogn; 2015 Mar; 18(2):475-83. PubMed ID: 25348065
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 16. A comparative study of the use of visual communicative signals in interactions between dogs (Canis familiaris) and humans and cats (Felis catus) and humans.
    Miklósi A; Pongrácz P; Lakatos G; Topál J; Csányi V
    J Comp Psychol; 2005 May; 119(2):179-86. PubMed ID: 15982161
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 17. Multimodal communication and audience directedness in the greeting behaviour of semi-captive African savannah elephants.
    Eleuteri V; Bates L; Rendle-Worthington J; Hobaiter C; Stoeger A
    Commun Biol; 2024 May; 7(1):472. PubMed ID: 38724671
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 18. Chimpanzees modify intentional gestures to coordinate a search for hidden food.
    Roberts AI; Vick SJ; Roberts SG; Menzel CR
    Nat Commun; 2014; 5():3088. PubMed ID: 24430433
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 19. The use of experimenter-given cues by South African fur seals (Arctocephalus pusillus).
    Scheumann M; Call J
    Anim Cogn; 2004 Oct; 7(4):224-30. PubMed ID: 15057598
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 20. Two-year-old children but not domestic dogs understand communicative intentions without language, gestures, or gaze.
    Moore R; Mueller B; Kaminski J; Tomasello M
    Dev Sci; 2015 Mar; 18(2):232-42. PubMed ID: 25041186
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Next]    [New Search]
    of 9.