These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.
122 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 24219086)
1. Base rates: both neglected and intuitive. Pennycook G; Trippas D; Handley SJ; Thompson VA J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn; 2014 Mar; 40(2):544-54. PubMed ID: 24219086 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
2. Are we good at detecting conflict during reasoning? Pennycook G; Fugelsang JA; Koehler DJ Cognition; 2012 Jul; 124(1):101-6. PubMed ID: 22575046 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
3. Intuitive reasoning about probability: theoretical and experimental analyses of the "problem of three prisoners". Shimojo S; Ichikawa SI Cognition; 1989 Jun; 32(1):1-24. PubMed ID: 2752704 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
4. Do smart people have better intuitions? Thompson VA; Pennycook G; Trippas D; Evans JSBT J Exp Psychol Gen; 2018 Jul; 147(7):945-961. PubMed ID: 29975089 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
5. When does information about causal structure improve statistical reasoning? McNair S; Feeney A Q J Exp Psychol (Hove); 2014; 67(4):625-45. PubMed ID: 23931633 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
6. Rule-based reasoning is fast and belief-based reasoning can be slow: Challenging current explanations of belief-bias and base-rate neglect. Newman IR; Gibb M; Thompson VA J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn; 2017 Jul; 43(7):1154-1170. PubMed ID: 28191989 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
7. Conflict monitoring in dual process theories of thinking. De Neys W; Glumicic T Cognition; 2008 Mar; 106(3):1248-99. PubMed ID: 17631876 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
9. When fast logic meets slow belief: Evidence for a parallel-processing model of belief bias. Trippas D; Thompson VA; Handley SJ Mem Cognit; 2017 May; 45(4):539-552. PubMed ID: 28028779 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
10. Logic feels so good-I like it! Evidence for intuitive detection of logicality in syllogistic reasoning. Morsanyi K; Handley SJ J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn; 2012 May; 38(3):596-616. PubMed ID: 22060275 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
11. Not that neglected! Base rates influence related and unrelated judgments. Białek M Acta Psychol (Amst); 2017 Jun; 177():10-16. PubMed ID: 28431299 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
12. The logic-bias effect: The role of effortful processing in the resolution of belief-logic conflict. Howarth S; Handley SJ; Walsh C Mem Cognit; 2016 Feb; 44(2):330-49. PubMed ID: 26390872 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
13. Fast and slow thinking: Electrophysiological evidence for early conflict sensitivity. Bago B; Frey D; Vidal J; Houdé O; Borst G; De Neys W Neuropsychologia; 2018 Aug; 117():483-490. PubMed ID: 30025789 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
14. The role of causal models in multiple judgments under uncertainty. Hayes BK; Hawkins GE; Newell BR; Pasqualino M; Rehder B Cognition; 2014 Dec; 133(3):611-20. PubMed ID: 25238316 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
15. The role of causality in judgment under uncertainty. Krynski TR; Tenenbaum JB J Exp Psychol Gen; 2007 Aug; 136(3):430-50. PubMed ID: 17696692 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
16. Background beliefs in Bayesian inference. Evans JS; Handley SJ; Over DE; Perham N Mem Cognit; 2002 Mar; 30(2):179-90. PubMed ID: 12035880 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]
17. Integration of base rates and new information in an abstract hypothesis-testing task. Rusconi P; Marelli M; Russo S; D'Addario M; Cherubini P Br J Psychol; 2013 May; 104(2):193-211. PubMed ID: 23560666 [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]