These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

119 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 24352697)

  • 1. Reversed better-than-average effect in direct comparisons of nonsocial stimuli depends on the set size.
    Niewiarowski J; Karyłowski JJ; Szutkiewicz-Szekalska K; Cypryańska M
    Mem Cognit; 2014 May; 42(4):639-47. PubMed ID: 24352697
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 2. Direct-comparison judgments: when and why above- and below-average effects reverse.
    Windschitl PD; Conybeare D; Krizan Z
    J Exp Psychol Gen; 2008 Feb; 137(1):182-200. PubMed ID: 18248136
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 3. Sample size bias in judgments of perceptual averages.
    Price PC; Kimura NM; Smith AR; Marshall LD
    J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn; 2014 Sep; 40(5):1321-31. PubMed ID: 24749965
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 4. Ensemble representations: effects of set size and item heterogeneity on average size perception.
    Marchant AP; Simons DJ; de Fockert JW
    Acta Psychol (Amst); 2013 Feb; 142(2):245-50. PubMed ID: 23376135
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 5. The role of similarity and dissimilarity focus in sequential judgments of physical attractiveness.
    Cypryanska M; Bedynska S; De Zavala AG
    J Soc Psychol; 2012; 152(3):266-9. PubMed ID: 22558823
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 6. The neural correlates of beauty comparison.
    Kedia G; Mussweiler T; Mullins P; Linden DE
    Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci; 2014 May; 9(5):681-8. PubMed ID: 23508477
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 7. Duration perception of visual and auditory oddball stimuli: does judgment task modulate the temporal oddball effect?
    Birngruber T; Schröter H; Ulrich R
    Atten Percept Psychophys; 2014 Apr; 76(3):814-28. PubMed ID: 24435899
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 8. Back view of beauty: a bias in attractiveness judgment.
    Yonemura K; Ono F; Watanabe K
    Perception; 2013; 42(1):95-102. PubMed ID: 23678619
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 9. Spatial attention is necessary for object-based attention: Evidence from temporal-order judgments.
    Donovan I; Pratt J; Shomstein S
    Atten Percept Psychophys; 2017 Apr; 79(3):753-764. PubMed ID: 28028777
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 10. The effect of nontemporal stimulus size on perceived duration as assessed by the method of reproduction.
    Rammsayer TH; Verner M
    J Vis; 2014 May; 14(5):17. PubMed ID: 24879864
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 11. How can observers use perceived size? Centroid versus mean-size judgments.
    Rodriguez-Cintron LM; Wright CE; Chubb C; Sperling G
    J Vis; 2019 Mar; 19(3):3. PubMed ID: 30884494
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 12. Eyelid-openness and mouth curvature influence perceived intelligence beyond attractiveness.
    Talamas SN; Mavor KI; Axelsson J; Sundelin T; Perrett DI
    J Exp Psychol Gen; 2016 May; 145(5):603-620. PubMed ID: 26913618
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 13. The moon size illusion does not improve perceptual judgments.
    Francis G; Cummins B; Kim J; Grzeczkowski L; Thunell E
    Conscious Cogn; 2019 Aug; 73():102754. PubMed ID: 31158723
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 14. The role of holistic processing in judgments of facial attractiveness.
    Abbas ZA; Duchaine B
    Perception; 2008; 37(8):1187-96. PubMed ID: 18853555
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 15. Body-scaled perception is subjected to adaptation when repetitively judging opportunities for grasping.
    Kim S; Frank TD
    Exp Brain Res; 2016 Sep; 234(9):2731-43. PubMed ID: 27220768
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 16. Orienting attention in visual space by nociceptive stimuli: investigation with a temporal order judgment task based on the adaptive PSI method.
    Filbrich L; Alamia A; Burns S; Legrain V
    Exp Brain Res; 2017 Jul; 235(7):2069-2079. PubMed ID: 28374087
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 17. Sequential biases on subjective judgments: Evidence from face attractiveness and ringtone agreeableness judgment.
    Huang J; He X; Ma X; Ren Y; Zhao T; Zeng X; Li H; Chen Y
    PLoS One; 2018; 13(6):e0198723. PubMed ID: 29889850
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 18. The whole is indeed more than the sum of its parts: perceptual averaging in the absence of individual item representation.
    Corbett JE; Oriet C
    Acta Psychol (Amst); 2011 Oct; 138(2):289-301. PubMed ID: 21903186
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 19. Perceived beauty of random texture patterns: A preference for complexity.
    Friedenberg J; Liby B
    Acta Psychol (Amst); 2016 Jul; 168():41-9. PubMed ID: 27155800
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 20. Becoming sexy: Contrapposto pose increases attractiveness ratings and modulates observers' brain activity.
    Pazhoohi F; Arantes J; Kingstone A; Pinal D
    Biol Psychol; 2020 Mar; 151():107842. PubMed ID: 31958547
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Next]    [New Search]
    of 6.