These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

133 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 24410679)

  • 41. The differing roles of comparison and contrast in children's categorization.
    Namy LL; Clepper LE
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2010 Nov; 107(3):291-305. PubMed ID: 20609449
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 42. Perseveration and the status of 3-year-olds' knowledge in a card-sorting task: evidence from studies involving congruent flankers.
    Jordan PL; Morton JB
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2012 Jan; 111(1):52-64. PubMed ID: 21862034
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 43. Removing shared information improves 3- and 4-year-olds' performance on a change-of-location explicit false belief task.
    Salter G; Breheny R
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2019 Nov; 187():104665. PubMed ID: 31409457
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 44. How do symbols affect 3- to 4-year-olds' executive function? Evidence from a reverse-contingency task.
    Apperly IA; Carroll DJ
    Dev Sci; 2009 Nov; 12(6):1070-82. PubMed ID: 19840061
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 45. Preschoolers' use of feedback for flexible behavior: insights from a computational model.
    Chevalier N; Dauvier B; Blaye A
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2009 Jul; 103(3):251-67. PubMed ID: 19394029
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 46. Executive function and the development of belief-desire psychology.
    Rakoczy H
    Dev Sci; 2010 Jul; 13(4):648-61. PubMed ID: 20590728
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 47. The development of children's information gathering: to look or to ask?
    Fitneva SA; Lam NH; Dunfield KA
    Dev Psychol; 2013 Mar; 49(3):533-42. PubMed ID: 23316769
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 48. Making Sense of the Cognitive Task of Medication Reconciliation Using a Card Sorting Task.
    Bitan Y; Parmet Y; Greenfield G; Teng S; Cook RI; Nunnally ME
    Hum Factors; 2019 Dec; 61(8):1315-1325. PubMed ID: 30912979
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 49. Testing the predictive power of cognitive atypicalities in autistic children: evidence from a 3-year follow-up study.
    Pellicano E
    Autism Res; 2013 Aug; 6(4):258-67. PubMed ID: 23495146
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 50. Task switching and novelty processing activate a common neural network for cognitive control.
    Barcelo F; Escera C; Corral MJ; Periáñez JA
    J Cogn Neurosci; 2006 Oct; 18(10):1734-48. PubMed ID: 17014377
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 51. The development of analogy making in children: cognitive load and executive functions.
    Thibaut JP; French R; Vezneva M
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2010 May; 106(1):1-19. PubMed ID: 20153481
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 52. Children's level of participation in a false-belief task, age, and theory of mind.
    Ritblatt SN
    J Genet Psychol; 2000 Mar; 161(1):53-64. PubMed ID: 10705584
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 53. When labels hurt but novelty helps: children's perseveration and flexibility in a card-sorting task.
    Yerys BE; Munakata Y
    Child Dev; 2006; 77(6):1589-607. PubMed ID: 17107448
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 54. Age-group differences in set-switching and set-maintenance on the Wisconsin Card Sorting Task.
    Huizinga M; van der Molen MW
    Dev Neuropsychol; 2007; 31(2):193-215. PubMed ID: 17488216
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 55. Children's understanding of inferential knowledge.
    Pillow BH
    J Genet Psychol; 1999 Dec; 160(4):419-28. PubMed ID: 10584320
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 56. Children's reasoning about the causal significance of the temporal order of events.
    McCormack T; Hoerl C
    Dev Psychol; 2005 Jan; 41(1):54-63. PubMed ID: 15656737
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 57. A developmental window into trade-offs in executive function: the case of task switching versus response inhibition in 6-year-olds.
    Blackwell KA; Chatham CH; Wiseheart M; Munakata Y
    Neuropsychologia; 2014 Sep; 62():356-64. PubMed ID: 24791710
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 58. Examining age-related differences in auditory attention control using a task-switching procedure.
    Lawo V; Koch I
    J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci; 2014 Mar; 69(2):237-44. PubMed ID: 23197343
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 59. Children's use of comparison and function in novel object categorization.
    Kimura K; Hunley SB; Namy LL
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2018 Jun; 170():161-176. PubMed ID: 29477094
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 60. Children's understanding of the modal expression of speaker certainty and uncertainty and its relation to the development of a representational theory of mind.
    Moore C; Pure K; Furrow D
    Child Dev; 1990 Jun; 61(3):722-30. PubMed ID: 2364747
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Previous]   [Next]    [New Search]
    of 7.