These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

195 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 24581047)

  • 1. Understanding the mapping between numerical approximation and number words: evidence from Williams syndrome and typical development.
    Libertus ME; Feigenson L; Halberda J; Landau B
    Dev Sci; 2014 Nov; 17(6):905-19. PubMed ID: 24581047
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 2. Egocentric and allocentric navigation strategies in Williams syndrome and typical development.
    Broadbent HJ; Farran EK; Tolmie A
    Dev Sci; 2014 Nov; 17(6):920-34. PubMed ID: 24702907
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 3. What makes counting count? Verbal and visuo-spatial contributions to typical and atypical number development.
    Ansari D; Donlan C; Thomas MS; Ewing SA; Peen T; Karmiloff-Smith A
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2003 May; 85(1):50-62. PubMed ID: 12742762
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 4. Mathematical skills in Williams syndrome: insight into the importance of underlying representations.
    O'Hearn K; Luna B
    Dev Disabil Res Rev; 2009; 15(1):11-20. PubMed ID: 19213012
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 5. Developmental profiles for multiple object tracking and spatial memory: typically developing preschoolers and people with Williams syndrome.
    O'Hearn K; Hoffman JE; Landau B
    Dev Sci; 2010 May; 13(3):430-440. PubMed ID: 20443964
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 6. The precision of mapping between number words and the approximate number system predicts children's formal math abilities.
    Libertus ME; Odic D; Feigenson L; Halberda J
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2016 Oct; 150():207-226. PubMed ID: 27348475
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 7. Mathematical skill in individuals with Williams syndrome: evidence from a standardized mathematics battery.
    O'Hearn K; Landau B
    Brain Cogn; 2007 Aug; 64(3):238-46. PubMed ID: 17482333
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 8. Geometric and featural systems, separable and combined: Evidence from reorientation in people with Williams syndrome.
    Ferrara K; Landau B
    Cognition; 2015 Nov; 144():123-33. PubMed ID: 26275835
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 9. From genes to brain development to phenotypic behavior: "dorsal-stream vulnerability" in relation to spatial cognition, attention, and planning of actions in Williams syndrome (WS) and other developmental disorders.
    Atkinson J; Braddick O
    Prog Brain Res; 2011; 189():261-83. PubMed ID: 21489394
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 10. Neuropsychological profile of Italians with Williams syndrome: an example of a dissociation between language and cognition?
    Vicari S; Bates E; Caselli MC; Pasqualetti P; Gagliardi C; Tonucci F; Volterra V
    J Int Neuropsychol Soc; 2004 Oct; 10(6):862-76. PubMed ID: 15637777
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 11. Comprehension of sarcasm, metaphor and simile in Williams syndrome.
    Godbee K; Porter M
    Int J Lang Commun Disord; 2013; 48(6):651-65. PubMed ID: 24165362
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 12. Spatial language in Williams syndrome: evidence for a special interaction?
    Lukács A; Pléh C; Racsmány M
    J Child Lang; 2007 May; 34(2):311-43. PubMed ID: 17542160
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 13. Comprehension of spatial language terms in Williams syndrome: evidence for an interaction between domains of strength and weakness.
    Phillips CE; Jarrold C; Baddeley AD; Grant J; Karmiloff-Smith A
    Cortex; 2004 Feb; 40(1):85-101. PubMed ID: 15070004
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 14. Language acquisition in special populations: a comparison between Down and Williams syndromes.
    Vicari S; Caselli MC; Gagliardi C; Tonucci F; Volterra V
    Neuropsychologia; 2002; 40(13):2461-70. PubMed ID: 12417473
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 15. Comprehension of spatial language in Williams syndrome: evidence for impaired spatial representation of verbal descriptions.
    Laing E; Jarrold C
    Clin Linguist Phon; 2007 Sep; 21(9):689-704. PubMed ID: 17701756
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 16. The development of metaphorical language comprehension in typical development and in Williams syndrome.
    Thomas MS; Van Duuren M; Purser HR; Mareschal D; Ansari D; Karmiloff-Smith A
    J Exp Child Psychol; 2010; 106(2-3):99-114. PubMed ID: 20236654
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 17. Consequences of severe visual-spatial deficits for reading acquisition: evidence from Williams syndrome.
    Dessalegn B; Landau B; Rapp B
    Neurocase; 2013 Aug; 19(4):328-47. PubMed ID: 22583550
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 18. Numerical abilities in Williams syndrome: dissociating the analogue magnitude system and verbal retrieval.
    Krajcsi A; Lukács A; Igács J; Racsmány M; Pléh C
    J Clin Exp Neuropsychol; 2009 May; 31(4):439-46. PubMed ID: 18618357
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 19. Object-based mental rotation and visual perspective-taking in typical development and Williams syndrome.
    Broadbent HJ; Farran EK; Tolmie A
    Dev Neuropsychol; 2014; 39(3):205-25. PubMed ID: 24742311
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 20. Children's mappings between number words and the approximate number system.
    Odic D; Le Corre M; Halberda J
    Cognition; 2015 May; 138():102-21. PubMed ID: 25721021
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Next]    [New Search]
    of 10.