These tools will no longer be maintained as of December 31, 2024. Archived website can be found here. PubMed4Hh GitHub repository can be found here. Contact NLM Customer Service if you have questions.


BIOMARKERS

Molecular Biopsy of Human Tumors

- a resource for Precision Medicine *

148 related articles for article (PubMed ID: 24730741)

  • 1. Taking aim at the Müller-Lyer goalkeeper illusion: an illusion bias in action that originates from the target not being optically specified.
    Shim J; van der Kamp J; Rigby BR; Lutz R; Poolton JM; Masters RS
    J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform; 2014 Jun; 40(3):1274-81. PubMed ID: 24730741
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 2. The human Müller-Lyer illusion in goalkeeping.
    van der Kamp J; Masters RS
    Perception; 2008; 37(6):951-4. PubMed ID: 18686713
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 3. Perception-action and the Müller-Lyer illusion: amplitude or endpoint bias?
    Glazebrook CM; Dhillon VP; Keetch KM; Lyons J; Amazeen E; Weeks DJ; Elliott D
    Exp Brain Res; 2005 Jan; 160(1):71-8. PubMed ID: 15300347
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 4. Visual illusions affect both movement planning and on-line control: a multiple cue position on bias and goal-directed action.
    Mendoza J; Hansen S; Glazebrook CM; Keetch KM; Elliott D
    Hum Mov Sci; 2005; 24(5-6):760-73. PubMed ID: 16223538
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 5. The effect of the Müller-Lyer illusion on the planning and control of manual aiming movements.
    Mendoza JE; Elliott D; Meegan DV; Lyons JL; Welsh TN
    J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform; 2006 Apr; 32(2):413-22. PubMed ID: 16634679
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 6. The neural mechanisms underlying the Müller-Lyer illusion and its interaction with visuospatial judgments.
    Weidner R; Fink GR
    Cereb Cortex; 2007 Apr; 17(4):878-84. PubMed ID: 16707733
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 7. Visuomotor 'immunity' to perceptual illusion: a mismatch of attentional demands cannot explain the perception-action dissociation.
    Dewar MT; Carey DP
    Neuropsychologia; 2006; 44(8):1501-8. PubMed ID: 16364378
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 8. Dissociations between vision for perception and vision for action depend on the relative availability of egocentric and allocentric information.
    Cañal-Bruland R; Voorwald F; Wielaard K; van der Kamp J
    Atten Percept Psychophys; 2013 Aug; 75(6):1206-14. PubMed ID: 23670269
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 9. When is grasping affected by the Müller-Lyer illusion? A quantitative review.
    Bruno N; Franz VH
    Neuropsychologia; 2009 May; 47(6):1421-33. PubMed ID: 19059422
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 10. Cross-modal processing in the occipito-temporal cortex: a TMS study of the Müller-Lyer illusion.
    Mancini F; Bolognini N; Bricolo E; Vallar G
    J Cogn Neurosci; 2011 Aug; 23(8):1987-97. PubMed ID: 20807050
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 11. Illusions in action: consequences of inconsistent processing of spatial attributes.
    Smeets JB; Brenner E; de Grave DD; Cuijpers RH
    Exp Brain Res; 2002 Nov; 147(2):135-44. PubMed ID: 12410328
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 12. The effect of the Müller-Lyer configuration on saccadic eye movements is not fully due to illusory perception.
    Chen J; Yang P; Chen Z
    J Neurophysiol; 2020 Sep; 124(3):856-867. PubMed ID: 32783573
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 13. Another look at the Müller-Lyer illusion: different gaze patterns in vision for action and perception.
    van Doorn H; van der Kamp J; de Wit M; Savelsbergh GJ
    Neuropsychologia; 2009 Feb; 47(3):804-12. PubMed ID: 19118565
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 14. The perception of interpersonal distance is distorted by the Müller-Lyer illusion.
    Bunce C; Gray KLH; Cook R
    Sci Rep; 2021 Jan; 11(1):494. PubMed ID: 33436801
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 15. Two illusions of perceived orientation: one fools all of the people some of the time; the other fools all of the people all of the time.
    Dyde RT; Milner AD
    Exp Brain Res; 2002 Jun; 144(4):518-27. PubMed ID: 12037636
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 16. The temporal dynamics of the Müller-Lyer illusion.
    Weidner R; Boers F; Mathiak K; Dammers J; Fink GR
    Cereb Cortex; 2010 Jul; 20(7):1586-95. PubMed ID: 19875676
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 17. Bistable illusory rebound motion: Event-related functional magnetic resonance imaging of perceptual states and switches.
    Hsieh PJ; Caplovitz GP; Tse PU
    Neuroimage; 2006 Aug; 32(2):728-39. PubMed ID: 16702003
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 18. The moon illusion and size-distance scaling--evidence for shared neural patterns.
    Weidner R; Plewan T; Chen Q; Buchner A; Weiss PH; Fink GR
    J Cogn Neurosci; 2014 Aug; 26(8):1871-82. PubMed ID: 24564459
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 19. Superadditivity of the Ebbinghaus and Müller-Lyer illusions depends on the method of comparison used.
    Foster RM; Franz VH
    Perception; 2014; 43(8):783-95. PubMed ID: 25549508
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

  • 20. A metanalysis of the effect of the Müller-Lyer illusion on saccadic eye movements: no general support for a dissociation of perception and oculomotor action.
    Bruno N; Knox PC; de Grave DD
    Vision Res; 2010 Dec; 50(24):2671-82. PubMed ID: 20858510
    [TBL] [Abstract][Full Text] [Related]  

    [Next]    [New Search]
    of 8.